Misplaced Pages

Dubai Dolphinarium

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.

Dubai Dolphinarium is a fully air-conditioned indoor dolphinarium in the Middle East , providing habitat to dolphins and seals , allowing the public to watch and interact with them through live shows and photo sessions. It is located at Creek Park near the Children's City within the vicinity of Oud Metha and Bur Dubai . Dubai Dolphinarium was opened on May 21, 2008 in partnership with Dubai Municipality , and is sponsored and supported by Dubai government to provide the general public with entertainment and also educate them regarding dolphins, seals and other marine organisms. It was also reflected that interacting with such friendly mammals like dolphins will motivate young generation to protect marine life and the environment.

#296703

123-580: Dubai Dolphinarium is a 5,000-square-meter (54,000 sq ft) modern indoor facility with around 1250 seating capacity for Dolphin & Seal Show. The Dolphins have their own private habitat area with 600 cubic metres (160,000 US gal) of sea water connected to the main arena pool. There is a separate medical pool and seal pool constructed considering well-being of these marine mammals. The dolphinarium complex also features group and kids activities, birthday parties for kids, school field trips, group events, swim with dolphins, mirror maze, bird show ,

246-421: A hoverfly , is harmless, while its model, such as a wasp , is harmful, and is avoided by the dupe, such as an insect-eating bird. Birds hunt by sight, so the mimicry in that case is visual, but in other cases mimicry may make use of any of the senses. Most types of mimicry, including Batesian, are deceptive, as the mimics are not harmful, but Müllerian mimicry , where different harmful species resemble each other,

369-413: A marine sponge on their rostrum , presumably to protect it when searching for food on the sandy sea bottom. This has only been observed in this bay (first in 1997), and is predominantly practiced by females. A 2005 study showed mothers most likely teach the behavior to their offspring, evincing culture (behavior learned from other species members). Mud plume feeding is a feeding technique performed by

492-458: A supergene for the development of butterfly color patterns. The model is supported by computational simulations of population genetics . The Batesian mimicry in Papilio polytes is controlled by the doublesex gene. Some mimicry is imperfect. Natural selection drives mimicry only far enough to deceive predators. For example, when predators avoid a mimic that imperfectly resembles a coral snake,

615-674: A change of water flow pattern from the infant which minimizes separation between the mother and infant, but also increases the mother's surface area and creates a drag for the swimmer. This also leaves less energy to use in swimming speed, foraging, and predator evasion. Adult males live mostly alone or in groups of two to three, and join pods for short periods of time. Adult females and young dolphins normally live in groups of up to 15 animals. Males give strong mutual support if other males help them, even if they are not friends. However, they live in fission-fusion societies of varying group size , within which individuals change associations, often on

738-418: A complex cluster of linked genes that cause large changes in morphology. The second step consists of selections on genes with smaller phenotypic effects, creating an increasingly close resemblance. This model is supported by empirical evidence that suggests that a few single point mutations cause large phenotypic effects, while numerous others produce smaller effects. Some regulatory elements collaborate to form

861-497: A concentrated food source. Mimicry In evolutionary biology , mimicry is an evolved resemblance between an organism and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. In the simplest case, as in Batesian mimicry , a mimic resembles a model, so as to deceive a dupe, all three being of different species. A Batesian mimic, such as

984-556: A daily or hourly basis. Group compositions are usually determined by sex, age, reproductive condition, familial relations and affiliation histories. In a dolphin community near Sarasota, Florida , the most common group types are adult females with their recent offspring, older subadults of both sexes, and adult males either alone or in bonded pairs. Smaller groups can join to form larger groups of 100 or more, and occasionally exceed 1,000. The social strategies of marine mammals such as bottlenose dolphins "provide interesting parallels" with

1107-416: A deceptive distraction display to lure predators away from their flightless young: When a man comes by chance upon a young brood [of partridges], and tries to catch them, the hen-bird rolls in front of the hunter, pretending to be lame: the man every moment thinks he is on the point of catching her, and so she draws him on and on, until every one of her brood has had time to escape; hereupon she returns to

1230-645: A family totally different in structure and metamorphosis from the Heliconidae , without examining them closely after capture. The German naturalist Fritz Müller also spent many years studying butterflies in the Amazon rainforest. He first published a journal article on mimicry in German in 1878, followed in 1879 by a paper to the Entomological Society of London (translated and presented by Ralph Meldola). He described

1353-424: A fish with its fluke, sometimes knocking it out of the water, using a strategy called "fish whacking". "Strand feeding" is an inherited feeding technique used by bottlenose dolphins near and around coastal regions of Georgia and South Carolina. When a pod finds a school of fish, they will circle the school and trap the fish in a mini whirlpool. Then, the dolphins will charge at the school and push their bodies up onto

SECTION 10

#1732851155297

1476-430: A genus of flatworm , matures in the digestive system of songbirds , their eggs then passing out of the bird in the faeces . They are then taken up by Succinea , a terrestrial snail. The eggs develop in this intermediate host , and must then find a suitable bird to mature in. Since the host birds do not eat snails, the sporocyst has another strategy to reach its host's intestine. They are brightly coloured and move in

1599-448: A human can: the dolphin can store 36 milliliters (ml) of oxygen per kg of body weight, compared with 20 ml per kg for humans. This is an adaptation to diving. The bottlenose dolphin typically rises to the surface to breathe through its blowhole two to three times per minute, although it can remain submerged for up to 20 minutes. Dolphins can breathe while "half-asleep". During the sleeping cycle, one brain hemisphere remains active, while

1722-553: A length of just over 4 m (13 ft). Its color varies considerably, is usually dark gray on the back and lighter gray on the flanks, but it can be bluish-grey, brownish-grey, or even nearly black, and is often darker on the back from the rostrum to behind the dorsal fin . This is called countershading and is a form of camouflage . Older dolphins sometimes have a few spots. Bottlenose dolphins can live for more than 40 years. Females typically live 5–10 years longer than males, with some females exceeding 60 years. This extreme age

1845-456: A mud-flat, forcing the fish on the mud-flat, as well. The dolphins then crawl around on their sides, consuming the fish they washed up on shore. This happens only during low tides. One type of feeding behavior seen in bottlenose dolphins is mud ring feeding . Bottlenose dolphins conflict with small-scale coastal commercial fisheries in some Mediterranean areas. Common bottlenose dolphins are probably attracted to fishing nets because they offer

1968-666: A practice known as "strand feeding". Groups of between two and six dolphins are regularly observed creating a bow wave to force the fish out of the water. The dolphins follow the fish, stranding themselves briefly, to eat their prey before twisting their bodies back and forth in order to slide back into the water. While initially documented in South Carolina and Georgia, strand feeding has also been observed in Louisiana, Texas, Baja California, Ecuador, and Australia. Some Mauritanian dolphins cooperate with human fishermen. The dolphins drive

2091-431: A pulsating fashion. A sporocyst-sac pulsates in the snail's eye stalks, coming to resemble an irresistible meal for a songbird. In this way, it can bridge the gap between hosts, allowing it to complete its life cycle. A nematode ( Myrmeconema neotropicum ) changes the colour of the abdomen of workers of the canopy ant Cephalotes atratus to make it appear like the ripe fruits of Hyeronima alchorneoides . It also changes

2214-402: A rare form of self-decoration and social object use called grass-wearing. Self-decoration by wearing grass appears to be an attention-getting device rather than purely play and varies from a single blade to large clusters of grass. John's Pass dolphins self-decorate with grass primarily when they form new social groups or engage in procreative activities. Grass-wearing behavior among these dolphins

2337-465: A restaurant providing quick and fun meals for kids and a mini 5-D cinema theater. Dubai Dolphinarium is home to bottlenose dolphins and Northern fur seals . These dolphins and seals were reportedly bought from a country belonging to the Commonwealth of Independent States , an alliance of 11 former Soviet republics . Various activities like kids summer camp and Dubai Summer Surprises are held at

2460-470: A result of high fish density around the floating cages in the farming area. Fish is one of the main items in the dolphin diet. They also eat shrimps, squid, mollusks, and cuttlefish, and only swallow the soft parts. They eat 22 pounds of fish a day. When they encounter a shoal of fish , they work as a team to herd them towards the shore to maximize the harvest. They also hunt alone, often targeting bottom-dwelling species. The bottlenose dolphin sometimes hits

2583-548: A reward. This reproductive mimicry may not be readily apparent as members of the same species may still exhibit some degree of sexual dimorphism . It is common in many species of Caricaceae . In Dodsonian mimicry, named after Calaway H. Dodson , the model belongs to a different species than the mimic. By resembling the model, a flower can lure its pollinators without offering nectar. The mechanism occurs in several orchids, including Epidendrum ibaguense which mimics flowers of Lantana camara and Asclepias curassavica , and

SECTION 20

#1732851155297

2706-732: A school of fish towards the shore, where humans await with nets. In the confusion of casting nets, the dolphins catch a large number of fish as well. Intraspecies cooperative foraging has also been observed. These behaviors may also be transmitted via teaching. Controversially, Rendell and Whitehead have proposed a structure for the study of cetacean culture. Similar cases have been observed in Laguna, Santa Catarina in Brazil since during 19th century as well. Near Adelaide , in South Australia , several bottlenose dolphins " tail-walk ", whereby they elevate

2829-452: A series of dimples known as vibrissal pits able to sense electric fields. Dolphins have sharp eyesight. The eyes are located at the sides of the head and have a tapetum lucidum , or reflecting membrane, at the back of the retina, which aids vision in dim light. Their horseshoe-shaped, double-slit pupils enable dolphins to have good vision both in air and underwater, despite the different indices of refraction of these media. When under water,

2952-404: A short time. The dolphin's search for food is aided by a form of sonar known as echolocation : it locates objects by producing sounds and listening for the echoes. Clicking sounds are emitted in a focused beam in front of the dolphin. When the clicking sounds hit an object in the water, like a fish or rock, they bounce off and come back to the dolphin as echoes. Echolocation tells the dolphins

3075-527: A situation where different species were each unpalatable to predators, and shared similar, genuine, warning signals. Bates found it hard to explain why this should be so, asking why they should need to mimic each other if both were harmful and could warn off predators on their own. Müller put forward the first mathematical model of mimicry for this phenomenon: if a common predator confuses the two species, individuals in both those species are more likely to survive, as fewer individuals of either species are killed by

3198-541: A small community of bottlenose dolphins over shallow seagrass beds (less than 1 m) in the Florida Keys in the United States. The behavior involves creation of a U-shaped plume of mud in the water column and then rushing through the plume to capture fish. Along the beaches and tidal marshes of South Carolina and Georgia in the United States, bottlenose dolphins cooperatively herd prey fish onto steep and sandy banks in

3321-493: A synonym of T. truncatus , while T. nuuanu was recognized as a subspecies. In general, genetic variation between populations is significant, even among nearby populations. As a result of this genetic variation, other distinct species currently considered to be populations of common bottlenose dolphin are possible. Much of the discussion and doubts about its taxonomy is related to the existence of two ecotypes of bottlenose dolphins in many part of its distribution. For example,

3444-617: A third distinct species was described, the Burrunan dolphin ( T. (aduncus) australis ), found in the Port Phillip and Gippsland Lakes areas of Victoria , Australia, after research showed it was distinct from T. truncatus and T. aduncus , both in morphology and genetics. Also, evidence has been accumulating to validate the existence of a separate species, Lahille's bottlenose dolphin, T. gephyreus , that occurs in coastal waters of Argentina, Uruguay and southern Brazil. Other sources accept

3567-461: A twig or flower, they commonly do so upside down and shift their rear wings repeatedly, causing antenna-like movements of the "tails" on their wings. Studies of rear-wing damage support the hypothesis that this strategy is effective in deflecting attacks from the insect's head. Aggressive mimicry is found in predators or parasites that share some of the characteristics of a harmless species, allowing them to avoid detection by their prey or host ;

3690-504: A uniquely identifying, frequency-modulated narrow-band signature vocalization ( signature whistle ). Signature whistles, which are in a higher frequency range than humans can hear, have an important role in facilitating mother–calf contact. In the Sarasota Dolphin Research Program's library of recordings were 19 female common bottlenose dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ) producing signature whistles both with and without

3813-499: Is honest , as when species of wasps and of bees all have genuinely aposematic warning coloration. More complex types may be bipolar, involving only two species, such as when the model and the dupe are the same; this occurs for example in aggressive mimicry , where a predator in wolf-in-sheep's-clothing style resembles its prey, allowing it to hunt undetected. Mimicry is not limited to animals; in Pouyannian mimicry , an orchid flower

Dubai Dolphinarium - Misplaced Pages Continue

3936-412: Is a sheep in wolf's clothing . Mimics are less likely to be found out (for example by predators) when in low proportion to their model. Such negative frequency-dependent selection applies in most forms of mimicry. Specifically, Batesian mimicry can only be maintained if the harm caused to the predator by eating a model outweighs the benefit of eating a mimic. The nature of learning is weighted in favor of

4059-457: Is a toothed whale in the genus Tursiops . They are common, cosmopolitan members of the family Delphinidae , the family of oceanic dolphins . Molecular studies show the genus contains three species : the common bottlenose dolphin ( Tursiops truncatus ), the Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin ( Tursiops aduncus ), and Tamanend's bottlenose dolphin ( Tursiops erebennus ). Others, like

4182-586: Is a combination of visual, by olfaction , and by touch. Vavilovian mimicry is found in weeds that come to share characteristics with a domesticated plant through unintentional selection . It is named after Russian botanist and geneticist Nikolai Vavilov . Selection against the weed may occur either by manually killing the weed, or by separating its seeds from those of the crop by winnowing . Vavilovian mimicry illustrates unintentional selection by man . Weeders do not want to select weeds and their seeds that look increasingly like cultivated plants, yet there

4305-463: Is a local behavioral tradition that could constitute a cultural difference from other communities. Some researchers hypothesize that the number of nerve cells ( neurons ) in the cortex of the brain predicts intelligence in mammals . A 2019 study estimated the number of neurons in the cerebral cortex of three common bottlenose dolphins and found numbers ranging from 11.7 to 15.2 billion neurons. The human average being approximately 16 billion, this

4428-441: Is a postulated form of automimicry ; where the model belongs to the same species as the mimic. This is the analogue of Batesian mimicry within a single species, and occurs when there is a palatability spectrum within a population. Examples include the monarch and the queen from the subfamily Danainae , which feed on milkweed species of varying toxicity. These species store toxins from its host plant, which are maintained even in

4551-414: Is also unclear. The model is usually another species, except in automimicry , where members of the species mimic other members, or other parts of their own bodies, and in inter-sexual mimicry, where members of one sex mimic members of the other. Many types of mimicry have been described. An overview of each follows, highlighting the similarities and differences between the various forms. Classification

4674-405: Is an evolved resemblance between an organism and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. Often, mimicry functions to protect from predators . Mimicry systems have three basic roles: a mimic, a model, and a dupe. When these correspond to three separate species, the system is called disjunct; when

4797-579: Is energetically expensive. Males have large testes in relation to their body size. During the breeding season , males compete for access to females. Such competition can take the form of fighting other males or of herding females to prevent access by other males. Male Sarasota Bay common bottlenose dolphins with the defensive advantages of male pair-bonding range more widely than unpaired males, and encounter more unrelated females. In Shark Bay, male Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins have been observed working in pairs or larger groups to follow and/or restrict

4920-638: Is followed by rapid conception (1–2 months), whereas conception is delayed (2–9 months) if calf loss occurs between January and July. Weaning ages ranged from 2.7 to 8.0 years, but 66.7% (42 calves) were weaned by their fourth birthday. Females tended to wean mid-pregnancy. Group size was unrelated to water depth or female reproductive success, but reproductive success was predicted by water depth. Shallow water may allow mothers and calves to detect and avoid predatory sharks. Alternatively, or additionally, prey density may be higher in shallow water compared to deep water. Georgetown University professor Janet Mann argues

5043-401: Is inherited through the female line in so-called gentes (gens, singular). Intraspecific brood parasitism, where a female lays in a conspecific's nest, as illustrated by the goldeneye duck ( Bucephala clangula ), do not involve mimicry The parasitic butterfly Phengaris rebeli parasitizes the ant species Myrmica schencki by releasing chemicals that fool the worker ants to believe that

Dubai Dolphinarium - Misplaced Pages Continue

5166-608: Is likely within the range found in the human population. Bottlenose dolphins have a lifespan of 40–60 years. Females can outlive males and live for 60 years or more. Dolphins start to reproduce aged 5 to 15 years. The bottlenose dolphin has a single blowhole located on the dorsal surface of the head consisting of a hole and a muscular flap. The flap is closed during muscle relaxation and opens during contraction. Dolphins are voluntary breathers, who must deliberately surface and open their blowholes to get air. They can store almost twice as much oxygen in proportion to their body weight as

5289-472: Is managed by a staff of 60 including mammal trainers and customer supporting staff. Dubai Dolphinarium had come under criticism from various animal welfare campaigners in the beginning saying, it would lead to suffering and high dolphin mortality. Activists also questioned the source of its black sea bottlenose dolphins , claiming that one of them was not born in captivity but rescued from fishermen's nets, and therefore should have been re-released. However

5412-434: Is no other option. For example, early barnyard grass, Echinochloa oryzoides , is a weed in rice fields and looks similar to rice; its seeds are often mixed in rice and have become difficult to separate through Vavilovian mimicry. Vavilovian mimics may eventually be domesticated themselves, as in the case of rye in wheat; Vavilov called these weed-crops secondary crops . Inter-sexual mimicry (a type of automimicry, as it

5535-787: Is not a typical characteristic of most members of the species). Numerous investigations of bottlenose dolphin intelligence have been conducted, examining mimicry , use of artificial language , object categorization, and self-recognition . They can use tools (sponging; using marine sponges to forage for food sources they normally could not access) and transmit cultural knowledge from generation to generation, and their considerable intelligence has driven interaction with humans. Bottlenose dolphins gained popularity from aquarium shows and television programs such as Flipper . They have also been trained by militaries to locate sea mines or detect and mark enemy divers. In some areas, they cooperate with local fishermen by driving fish into their nets and eating

5658-461: Is often based on function with respect to the mimic (e.g., avoiding harm). Some cases may belong to more than one class, e.g., automimicry and aggressive mimicry are not mutually exclusive, as one describes the species relationship between model and mimic, while the other describes the function for the mimic (obtaining food). The terminology used has been debated, as classifications have differed or overlapped; attempts to clarify definitions have led to

5781-449: Is pollinated by monarch butterflies and perhaps hummingbirds . Brood parasitism or Kirbyan mimicry is a two species system where a brood parasite mimics its host. Cuckoos are a canonical example; the female cuckoo has its offspring raised by a bird of a different species, cutting down the biological mother's parental investment . The ability to lay eggs that mimic the host eggs is the key adaptation . The adaptation to different hosts

5904-504: Is rare and less than 2% of all Bottlenose dolphins will live longer than 60 years. Bottlenose dolphins can jump to a height of 6 metres (20 feet) in the air. Their elongated upper and lower jaws form what is called a rostrum, or snout, which gives the animal its common name. The real, functional nose is the blowhole on top of its head; the nasal septum is visible when the blowhole is open. Bottlenose dolphins have 18 to 28 conical teeth on each side of each jaw. The flukes (lobes of

6027-473: Is the mimic, resembling a female bee, its model; the dupe is the male bee of the same species, which tries to copulate with the flower, enabling it to transfer pollen, so the mimicry is again bipolar. In automimicry , another bipolar system, model and mimic are the same, as when blue lycaenid butterflies have 'tails' or eyespots on their wings that mimic their own heads, misdirecting predator dupes to strike harmlessly. Many other types of mimicry exist. Use of

6150-416: Is to mimic a mutualistic symbiont of the prey. Cleaner fish eat parasites and dead skin from client fish. Some allow the cleaner to venture inside their body to hunt these parasites. However, the sabre-toothed blenny or false cleanerfish ( Aspidontus taeniatus ) mimics the bluestreak cleaner wrasse ( Labroides dimidiatus ), which is recognized by other fishes as a cleaner. The false cleanerfish resembles

6273-466: Is unlike Müllerian mimicry, where the most harmful species is the model. But if a predator dies on its first encounter with a deadly snake, it has no occasion to learn to recognize the snake's warning signals. There would then be no advantage for an extremely deadly snake in being aposematic: any predator that attacked it would be killed before it could learn to avoid the deadly prey, so the snake would be better off being camouflaged to avoid attacks. But if

SECTION 50

#1732851155297

6396-421: Is widely accepted that mimicry evolves as a positive adaptation. The lepidopterist and novelist Vladimir Nabokov however argued that although natural selection might stabilize a "mimic" form, it would not be necessary to create it. The most widely accepted model used to explain the evolution of mimicry in butterflies is the two-step hypothesis. The first step involves mutation in modifier genes that regulate

6519-429: Is within a single species) occurs when individuals of one sex in a species mimic members of the opposite sex to facilitate sneak mating . An example is the three male forms of the marine isopod Paracerceis sculpta . Alpha males are the largest and guard a harem of females. Beta males mimic females and manage to enter the harem of females without being detected by the alpha males allowing them to mate. Gamma males are

6642-529: The Burrunan dolphin ( Tursiops (aduncus) australis ), may be alternately considered their own species or be subspecies of T. aduncus . Bottlenose dolphins inhabit warm and temperate seas worldwide, being found everywhere except for the Arctic and Antarctic Circle regions. Their name derives from the Latin tursio (dolphin) and truncatus for the truncated teeth (the type specimen was old and had worn down teeth; this

6765-576: The lichens of the oak on which I took it. The spectre tribe ( Phasma ) go still further in this mimicry, representing a small branch with its spray. The English naturalist Henry Walter Bates worked for several years on butterflies in the Amazon rainforest . Returning home, he described multiple forms of mimicry in an 1862 paper at the Linnean Society in London, and then in his 1863 book The Naturalist on

6888-443: The turkey vulture . It flies amongst the vultures, effectively camouflaged as a vulture which poses no threat to the hawk's prey. It hunts by suddenly breaking from the formation and ambushing its prey. Parasites can be aggressive mimics, though the situation is somewhat different from those outlined previously. They can mimic their hosts' natural prey, allowing themselves to be eaten as a pathway into their host. Leucochloridium ,

7011-454: The 1950s, no "dolphin language" has been found. However, Herman , Richards, and Wolz demonstrated comprehension of an artificial language by two bottlenose dolphins (named Akeakamai and Phoenix) in the period of skepticism toward animal language following Herbert Terrace 's critique. Cognitive abilities that have been investigated include concept formation , sensory skills, and mental representations. Such research has been ongoing since

7134-485: The 1970s. This includes: acoustic and behavioral mimicry, comprehension of novel sequences in an artificial language , memory , monitoring of self behavior, discrimination and matching, comprehension of symbols for various body parts, comprehension of pointing gestures and gaze (as made by dolphins or humans), mirror self-recognition , and numerical values. At least some wild bottlenose dolphins use tools. In Shark Bay , off Western Australia , dolphins place

7257-484: The Dubai Dolphinarium every year during summer holidays. Dubai Dolphinarium in association with Dubai Municipality organized an Autism Awareness Day from various autism care centres and training centres for children with special needs on April 28, 2011. Dubai Dolphinarium welcomed its one millionth visitor during March, 2012 and various promotional activities were featured during this period. This establishment

7380-562: The Eastern Tropical Pacific bottlenose dolphin ( T. t. nuuanu ). The IUCN, on their Red List of endangered species, currently recognises only two species of bottlenose dolphins. The American Society of Mammalogists also recognizes only two species. While acknowledging the studies describing T. australis , it classifies it within T. aduncus . Some recent genetic evidence suggests the Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin belongs in

7503-546: The Pacific bottlenose dolphin ( T. t. gillii or T. gillii ), that inhabits the Pacific, and has a black line from the eye to the forehead. T. gillii , first described in 1873, is currently considered a junior synonym of T. truncatus . Additionally, T. nuuanu was described in 1911 for bottlenose dolphins along the Pacific coast in Central America. An analysis of T. gillii and T. nuuanu specimens supported T. gillii as

SECTION 60

#1732851155297

7626-556: The River Amazons . The term "Batesian mimicry" has since been used in his honour, its usage becoming restricted to the situation in which a harmless mimic gains protection from its predators by resembling a distasteful model. Among the observations in Bates's 1862 paper is the statement: I was never able to distinguish the Leptalides from the species they imitated, although they belong to

7749-415: The adult. As levels of toxin vary depending on diet, some individuals are more toxic than the rest, which profit from the toxicity of those individuals, just as hoverflies benefit from mimicking well-defended wasps. One form of automimicry is where one part of an organism's body resembles another part. For example, the tails of some snakes resemble their heads; they move backwards when threatened and present

7872-444: The advantage of both organisms that share a resemblance, in which case it is mutualistic ; or it can be to the detriment of one, making it parasitic or competitive . The evolutionary convergence between groups is driven by the selective action of a dupe. Birds, for example, use sight to identify palatable insects, whilst avoiding noxious ones. Over time, palatable insects may evolve to resemble noxious ones, making them mimics and

7995-408: The back of the head, misleading predators into reacting as though they were the subject of an aggressive stare. Many insects have filamentous "tails" at the ends of their wings and patterns of markings on the wings themselves. These combine to create a "false head". This misdirects predators such as birds and jumping spiders . Spectacular examples occur in the hairstreak butterflies; when perching on

8118-668: The behaviour has continued through generations until 2022. A study conducted by the University of Chicago showed that bottlenose dolphins can remember whistles of other dolphins they had lived with after 20 years of separation. Each dolphin has a unique whistle that functions like a name, allowing the marine mammals to keep close social bonds. The new research shows that dolphins have the longest memory yet known in any species other than humans . The bottlenose dolphins of John's Pass in Boca Ciega Bay, St. Petersburg, Florida , exhibit

8241-462: The behaviour of the ant so that the gaster (rear part) is held raised. This presumably increases the chances of the ant being eaten by birds. Reproductive mimicry occurs when the actions of the dupe directly aid in the mimic's reproduction . This is common in plants with deceptive flowers that do not provide the reward they seem to offer and it may occur in Papua New Guinea fireflies, in which

8364-522: The brain. Bottlenose dolphins are able to detect salty, sweet, bitter ( quinine sulphate ), and sour ( citric acid ) tastes, but this has not been well-studied. Anecdotally, some individuals in captivity have been noted to have preferences for food fish types, although it is not clear if taste mediates this preference. In 2022, a study at the University of St Andrews in Scotland found that dolphins were able to identify their "friends" and family members by

8487-445: The caterpillar larvae are ant larvae. This enables the larvae to be brought directly into the ant's nest. In Pouyannian mimicry, a flower mimics a female of a certain insect species, inducing the males of that species to try to copulate with the flower. This is much like aggressive mimicry in fireflies, but with a more benign outcome for the pollinator. The mechanism is named after Maurice-Alexandre Pouyanne , who first described

8610-776: The child to pay attention, long-term bonding, and promoting the development of lifelong vocal learning , with parallels in these bottlenose dolphins in an example of convergent evolution . Researchers from the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute (BDRI), based in Sardinia (Italy) have now shown whistles and burst pulsed sounds are vital to the animals' social life and mirror their behaviors. The tonal whistle sounds (the most melodious ones) allow dolphins to stay in contact with each other (above all, mothers and offspring), and to coordinate hunting strategies. The burst-pulsed sounds (which are more complex and varied than

8733-402: The cleaner, and mimics the cleaner's "dance". Once it is allowed close to the client, it attacks, biting off a piece of its fin before fleeing. Fish wounded in this fashion soon learn to distinguish mimic from model, but because the similarity is close they also become much more cautious of the model. A mechanism that does not involve any luring is seen in the zone-tailed hawk , which resembles

8856-515: The coastal form while the offshore form was retained in T. truncatus . The Society for Marine Mammalogy 's Committee on Taxonomy presently recognizes three species of bottlenose dolphin: T. truncatus , T. aduncus , and T. erebennus . They also recognize three subspecies of common bottlenose dolphin in addition to the nominotypical subspecies : the Black Sea bottlenose dolphin ( T. t. ponticus ), Lahille's bottlenose dolphin ( T. t. gephyreus ), and

8979-512: The dolphins, which caused Instagram to issue a warning to whoever had searched “#dubaidolphinarium” on the app. An Instagram user, @aurelia_1534 posted a video captured at Dubai Dolphinarium, depicting a woman trainer bouncing on a dolphin’s top. However, the video was immediately taken down due to increasing condemnation from the viewers, who called the trainer’s behaviour “unethical” and “unprofessional”. Bottlenose dolphin The bottlenose dolphin

9102-463: The eyeball's lens serves to focus light, whereas in the in-air environment, the typically bright light serves to contract the specialized pupil, resulting in sharpness from a smaller aperture (similar to a pinhole camera ). By contrast, a bottlenose's sense of smell is poor, because its blowhole, the analog to the nose, is closed when underwater and it opens only for breathing. Like other toothed whales , it has no olfactory nerves or olfactory lobe in

9225-449: The fact that the Tursiops dolphins might consist of more than one species, as there is extensive variation in color and morphology along its range. In the past, most studies used morphology to evaluate differences between and within species, but in the late 20th century, combining morphological and molecular genetics allowed much greater insight into this previously intractable problem. Since

9348-648: The females are not in their estrous cycles and cannot produce young, suggesting they may mate for pleasure . The gestation period averages 12 months. Births can occur at any time of year, although peaks occur in warmer months. The young are born in shallow water, sometimes assisted by a (possibly male) "midwife", and usually only a single calf is born. Twins are possible, but rare. Newborn bottlenose dolphins are 0.8 to 1.4 m (2.6 to 4.6 ft) long and weigh 9 to 30 kg (20 to 66 lb), with Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin infants being generally smaller than common bottlenose dolphin infants. To accelerate nursing,

9471-600: The fish that escape. Some encounters with humans are harmful to the dolphins: people hunt them for food, and dolphins are killed inadvertently as a bycatch of tuna fishing and by getting caught in crab traps . Bottlenose dolphins have the third largest encephalization levels of any mammal on Earth (humans have the largest, followed by Northern Right whale dolphins ), sharing close ratios with those of humans and other cetaceans , while being twice as high of other great apes . This more than likely contributes to their high intelligence . Scientists have been long aware of

9594-537: The genital slit. The ability to stow their reproductive organs (especially in males) allows for maximum hydrodynamics. The breeding season produces significant physiological changes in males. At that time, the testes enlarge, enabling them to hold more sperm. Large amounts of sperm allow a male to wash away the previous suitor's sperm, while leaving some of his own for fertilization. Also, sperm concentration markedly increases. Having less sperm for out-of-season social mating means it wastes less. This suggests sperm production

9717-590: The genus Stenella , since it is more like the Atlantic spotted dolphin ( Stenella frontalis ) than the common bottlenose dolphin. However, more recent studies indicate that this is a consequence of reticulate evolution (such as past hybridization between Stenella and ancestral Tursiops ) and incomplete lineage sorting , and thus support T . truncatus and T. aduncus belonging to the same genus. Bottlenose dolphins have been known to hybridize with other dolphin species. Hybrids with Risso's dolphin occur both in

9840-585: The genus Thaumoctopus (the mimic octopus ) are able to intentionally alter their body shape and coloration to resemble dangerous sea snakes or lionfish . In the Amazon, the helmeted woodpecker ( Dryocopus galeatus ), a rare species which lives in the Atlantic Forest of Brazil, Paraguay, and Argentina, has a similar red crest, black back, and barred underside to two larger woodpeckers: Dryocopus lineatus and Campephilus robustus . This mimicry reduces attacks on D. galeatus . Batesian mimicry occurs in

9963-462: The late 1990s and early 2000s, most researchers acknowledged the existence of two species: the common bottlenose dolphin ( T. truncatus ), found in coastal and oceanic habitats of most tropical to temperate oceans, and the Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin ( T. aduncus ), that lives in coastal waters around India , northern Australia, South China, the Red Sea , and the eastern coast of Africa. In 2011,

10086-426: The management has countered these criticisms stating that the dolphins in Dubai Dolphinarium are third generation dolphins who were born in artificial conditions and are perfectly happy in their current environment and role, and are healthier all year round than their wild counterparts. In 2019, Dubai Dolphinarium came under heavy waves of criticism after a trainer posted on Instagram a video of them sitting on one of

10209-447: The mating signals of females of the genus Photinus . Male fireflies from several different genera are attracted to these " femmes fatales ", and are captured and eaten. Each female has a repertoire of signals matching the delay and duration of the flashes of the female of the corresponding species. Some carnivorous plants may be able to increase their rate of capturing insect prey through mimicry. A different aggressive strategy

10332-417: The milk snakes and the deadly coral snakes are mimics, while the false coral snakes are the model. In Wasmannian mimicry , the mimic resembles a model that it lives along with in a nest or colony. Most of the models here are eusocial insects, principally ants. Gilbertian mimicry is bipolar, involving only two species. The potential host (or prey) drives away its parasite (or predator) by mimicking it,

10455-415: The mimic and the model benefit from the interaction, which could thus be classified as mutualism . The signal receiver also benefits by this system, despite being deceived about species identity, as it is able to generalize the pattern to potentially harmful encounters. The distinction between mimic and model that is clear in Batesian mimicry is also blurred. Where one species is scarce and another abundant,

10578-410: The mimic is sufficiently protected. Convergent evolution is an alternative explanation for why coral reef fish have come to resemble each other; the same applies to benthic marine invertebrates such as sponges and nudibranchs . In its broadest definition, mimicry can include non-living models. The specific terms masquerade and mimesis are sometimes used when the models are inanimate, and

10701-400: The mimicry's purpose is crypsis . For example, animals such as flower mantises , planthoppers , comma and geometer moth caterpillars resemble twigs, bark, leaves, bird droppings or flowers. Many animals bear eyespots , which are hypothesized to resemble the eyes of larger animals. They may not resemble any specific organism's eyes, and whether or not animals respond to them as eyes

10824-425: The mimics, for a predator that has a bad first experience with a model tends to avoid anything that looks like it for a long time, and does not re-sample soon to see whether the initial experience was a false negative. However, if mimics become more abundant than models, then the probability of a young predator having a first experience with a mimic increases. Batesian systems are therefore most likely to be stable where

10947-418: The model is more abundant than the mimic. There are many Batesian mimics among butterflies and moths . Consul fabius and Eresia eunice imitate unpalatable Heliconius butterflies such as H. ismenius . Limenitis arthemis imitate the poisonous pipevine swallowtail ( Battus philenor ). Several palatable moths produce ultrasonic click calls to mimic unpalatable tiger moths. Octopuses of

11070-419: The mother can eject milk from her mammary glands . The calf suckles for 18 months to up to 8 years, and continues to closely associate with its mother for several years after weaning. Females sexually mature at ages 5–13, males at ages 9–14. Females reproduce every two to six years. Reproduction is moderately seasonal (September–January), peaking from October to December. Calf loss between August and December

11193-432: The movement of a female for weeks at a time, waiting for her to become sexually receptive . These coalitions, also known as male reproductive alliances , will fight with other coalitions for control of females. Humans and bottlenose dolphins are the only species that share this type of "gang formation" habit as a form of cooperation. Mating occurs belly to belly. Dolphins have been observed engaging in intercourse when

11316-412: The nest and calls the young back. The behaviour is recognised as a form of mimicry by biologists. In 1823, Kirby and Spence, in their book An Introduction to Entomology , used the term "mimicry" informally to depict the way that the structure and coloration of some insects resembled objects in their environments: A jumping bug, very similar to the one figured by Schellenberg , also much resembles

11439-558: The noxious ones models. Models do not have to be more abundant than mimics. In the case of mutualism, each model is also a mimic; all such species can be called "co-mimics". Many harmless species such as hoverflies are Batesian mimics of strongly defended species such as wasps, while many such well-defended species form Müllerian mimicry rings of co-mimics. In the evolution of wasp-like appearance, it has been argued that insects evolve to masquerade wasps since predatory wasps do not attack each other, and that this mimetic resemblance has had

11562-564: The other hemisphere shuts down. The active hemisphere handles surfacing and breathing behavior. The daily sleeping cycle lasts for about 8 hours, in increments of minutes to hours. During the sleeping cycle, they remain near the surface, swimming slowly or "logging", and occasionally closing one eye. Both sexes have genital slits on the underside of their bodies. The male can retract and conceal his penis through his slit. The female's slit houses her vagina and anus. Females have two mammary slits, each housing one nipple , one on each side of

11685-411: The partial replacement of old terms with new ones. Mimicry is defensive or protective when organisms are able to avoid harmful encounters by deceiving enemies into treating them as something else. In Batesian mimicry, the mimic resembles the model, but does not have the attribute that makes it unprofitable to predators (e.g., unpalatability, or the ability to sting). In other words, a Batesian mimic

11808-400: The pattern seen in many flowers known as nectar guides . Spiders change their web day to day, which can be explained by the ability of bees to remember web patterns. Another case is where males are lured towards what seems to be a sexually receptive female. The model in this situation is the same species as the dupe. Female fireflies of the genus Photuris emit light signals that mimic

11931-424: The phenomenon. It is most common in orchids, which mimic females of the order Hymenoptera (generally bees and wasps), and may account for around 60% of pollinations. Depending on the morphology of the flower, a pollen sac called a pollinium is attached to the head or abdomen of the male. This is then transferred to the stigma of the next flower the male tries to inseminate, resulting in pollination. The mimicry

12054-456: The plant kingdom, where the chameleon vine adapts its leaf shape and colour to match that of the plant it is climbing. In Müllerian mimicry, two or more species have similar warning or aposematic signals and both share genuine anti-predation attributes (e.g. being unpalatable), as first described in Heliconius butterflies. This type of mimicry is unique in several respects. Firstly, both

12177-505: The point of hatching. The butterflies avoid laying eggs near existing ones, reducing intraspecific competition between caterpillars, which are also cannibalistic , so those that lay on vacant leaves provide their offspring with a greater chance of survival. The stipules thus appear to have evolved as Gilbertian mimics of butterfly eggs, under selection pressure from these caterpillars. Browerian mimicry, named after Lincoln P. Brower and Jane Van Zandt Brower who first described it in 1967,

12300-408: The predator first learnt to avoid a less deadly warning-coloured snake, the deadly species could profit by mimicking the less dangerous snake. Some harmless milk snakes ( Lampropeltis triangulum ), the moderately toxic false coral snakes ( Erythrolamprus aesculapii ), and the deadly coral snakes ( Micrurus ) all have a red background color with black and white/yellow rings. In this system, both

12423-435: The predator with the tail, improving their chances of escape without fatal harm. Some fishes have eyespots near their tails, and when mildly alarmed swim slowly backwards, presenting the tail as a head. Some insects such as some lycaenid butterflies have tail patterns and appendages of various degrees of sophistication that promote attacks at the rear rather than at the head. Several species of pygmy owl bear "false eyes" on

12546-542: The predator. The term Müllerian mimicry , named in his honour, has since been used for this mutualistic form of mimicry. Müller wrote that The resemblance of the genera named [ Ituna and Thyridia ] is the more worthy of notice since it occurs between insects both belonging to the group of butterflies which are protected by distastefulness. The explanation which applies in ordinary cases of [Batesian] mimicry—and no other has, so far as I know, been offered—cannot obtain for this imitation among protected species. Mimicry

12669-441: The presence of their dependent calf. In all 19 cases, the mother dolphin changed the same signature whistle when the calf was present, by reaching a higher frequency, or using a wider frequency range. Similarly, humans use higher fundamental frequencies and a wider pitch range to inflect child–directed speech (CDS). This has rarely been discovered in other species. The researchers stated that CDS benefits for humans are cueing

12792-435: The rare species can be said to be the mimic. When both are present in similar numbers, however, it makes more sense to speak of each as a co-mimic than of distinct 'mimic' and 'model' species, as their warning signals tend to converge. Also, the mimetic species may exist on a continuum from harmless to highly noxious, so Batesian mimicry grades smoothly into Müllerian convergence. Emsleyan or Mertensian mimicry describes

12915-507: The reverse of host-parasite aggressive mimicry. It was coined by Pasteur as a phrase for such rare mimicry systems, and is named after the American ecologist Lawrence E. Gilbert who described it in 1975. The classical instance of Gilbertian mimicry is in the plant genus Passiflora , which is grazed by the micropredator larvae of some Heliconius butterflies. The host plants have evolved stipules that mimic mature Heliconius eggs near

13038-432: The roles are taken by just two species, the system is called bipolar. Mimicry evolves if a dupe (such as a predator) perceives a mimic (such as a palatable prey) as a model (the organism it resembles), and is deceived to change its behaviour to the mimic's selective advantage. The resemblances can be via any sensory modality, including any combination of visual, acoustic, chemical, tactile, or electric. Mimicry may be to

13161-617: The sensitivity of the sound receptor.) The interclick interval also decreases as the animal nears the target. Evidently, the dolphin waits for each click's echo before clicking again. Echolocation details, such as signal strength, spectral qualities, and discrimination, are well understood by researchers. Bottlenose dolphins are also able to extract shape information, suggesting they are able to form an "echoic image" or sound picture of their targets. They also have electroreception . The calves are born with two slender rows of whiskers along their snout, which fall off soon after birth, leaving behind

13284-411: The shape, size, speed, distance, and location of the object. To hear the returning echo, they have two small ear openings behind the eyes, but most sound waves are transmitted to the inner ear through the lower jaw. As the object of interest is approached, the echo becomes booming, and the dolphins adjust by decreasing the intensity of the emitted sounds. (This contrasts with bats and sonar, which reduce

13407-468: The signal of Pteroptyx effulgens is used by P. tarsalis to form aggregations to attract females. Other forms of mimicry have a reproductive component, such as Vavilovian mimicry involving seeds, vocal mimicry in birds, and aggressive and Batesian mimicry in brood parasite-host systems. Bakerian mimicry, named after Herbert G. Baker , is a form of automimicry where female flowers mimic male flowers of their own species, cheating pollinators out of

13530-460: The social strategies of elephants and chimpanzees. Bottlenose dolphins studied by Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute researchers off the island of Sardinia show random social behavior while feeding, and their social behavior does not depend on feeding activity. In Sardinia, the presence of a floating marine fin-fish farm has been linked to a change in bottlenose dolphin distribution as

13653-450: The strategy resembles a wolf in sheep's clothing , though no conscious deceptive intent is involved. The mimic may resemble the prey or host itself, or another organism that does not threaten the prey or host. Several spiders use aggressive mimicry to lure prey. Species such as the silver argiope ( Argiope argentata ) employ prominent patterns in the middle of their webs, such as zigzags. These may reflect ultraviolet light, and mimic

13776-526: The strong personal behavior among male calves is about bond formation and benefits the species in an evolutionary context. She cites studies showing these dolphins as adults are inseparable, and that early bonds aid protection, as well as in locating females. Female bottlenose dolphins have to expend additional energy in carrying out parental care, e.g. , infant-carrying behavior. Dolphins do not physically hold their infants but line up in an echelon position with infants swimming beside them. This position creates

13899-534: The tail) and dorsal fin are formed of dense connective tissue and do not contain bone or muscle . The dorsal fin usually shows phenotypic variations that help discriminate among populations. The animal propels itself by moving the flukes up and down. The pectoral flippers (at the sides of the body) are for steering; they contain bones homologous to the forelimbs of land mammals. A bottlenose dolphin discovered in Japan has two additional pectoral fins, or "hind legs", at

14022-640: The tail, about the size of a human's pair of hands. Scientists believe a mutation caused the ancient trait to reassert itself as a form of atavism . In colder waters, they have more body fat and blood, and are more suited to deeper diving. Typically, 18%–20% of their body weight is blubber. Most research in this area has been restricted to the North Atlantic Ocean . Bottlenose dolphins typically swim at 5 to 11 km/h (1.4 to 3.1 m/s), but are capable of bursts of up to 29 to 35 km/h (8.1 to 9.7 m/s). The higher speeds can only be sustained for

14145-481: The taste of their urine in the water. Bottlenose dolphins communicate through burst pulsed sounds, whistles, and body language. Examples of body language include leaping out of the water, snapping jaws, slapping the tail on the surface and butting heads. Sounds and gestures help keep track of other dolphins in the group, and alert other dolphins to danger and nearby food. Lacking vocal cords , they produce sounds using six air sacs near their blow hole. Each animal has

14268-427: The two ecotypes of the common bottlenose dolphin within the western North Atlantic are represented by the shallower water or coastal ecotype and the more offshore ecotype. Their ranges overlap, but they have been shown to be genetically distinct. In 2022, Costa et al. established morphologic, genetic, and evolutionary divergence between the two ecotypes in the western North Atlantic, resurrecting Tursiops erebennus for

14391-508: The unusual case where a deadly prey mimics a less dangerous species. It was first proposed by M. G. Emsley in 1966 as a possible explanation for how a predator can learn to avoid a very dangerous aposematic animal, such as a coral snake , when the predator is very likely to die, making learning unlikely. The theory was developed by the German biologist Wolfgang Wickler who named it after the German herpetologist Robert Mertens . The scenario

14514-437: The upper part of their bodies vertically out of the water, and propel themselves along the surface with powerful tail movements. Tail-walking mostly arises via human training in dolphinaria. In the 1980s, a female from the local population was kept at a local dolphinarium for three weeks, and the scientist suggests she copied the tail-walking behavior from other dolphins. Two other wild adult female dolphins copied it from her, and

14637-736: The useful side-effect of deterring vertebrate predators. Mimicry can result in an evolutionary arms race if mimicry negatively affects the model, in which case the model can evolve a different appearance from the mimic. Mimics may have different models for different life cycle stages, or they may be polymorphic , with different individuals imitating different models, as occurs in Heliconius butterflies. Models tend to be relatively closely related to their mimics, but mimicry can be of vastly different species, for example when spiders mimic ants. Most known mimics are insects, though many other examples including vertebrates , plants, and fungi exist. It

14760-451: The whistles) are used "to avoid physical aggression in situations of high excitement", such as when they are competing for the same piece of food, for example. The dolphins emit these strident sounds when in the presence of other individuals moving towards the same prey. The "least dominant" one soon moves away to avoid confrontation. Other communication uses about 30 distinguishable sounds, and although famously proposed by John C. Lilly in

14883-604: The wild and in captivity. The best known hybrid is the wholphin , a false killer whale -bottlenose dolphin hybrid. The wholphin is fertile, and two currently live at the Sea Life Park in Hawaii. The first was born in 1985 to a female bottlenose. Wholphins also exist in the wild. In captivity, a bottlenose dolphin and a rough-toothed dolphin hybridized. A common dolphin -bottlenose dolphin hybrid born in captivity lives at SeaWorld California. Other hybrids live in captivity around

15006-531: The word mimicry dates to 1637. It derives from the Greek term mimetikos , "imitative", in turn from mimetos , the verbal adjective of mimeisthai , "to imitate". "Mimicry" was first used in zoology by the English entomologists William Kirby and William Spence in 1823. Originally used to describe people, "mimetic" was used in zoology from 1851. Aristotle wrote in his History of Animals that partridges use

15129-712: The world and in the wild, such as a bottlenose dolphin-Atlantic spotted dolphin hybrid. Bottlenose dolphins appeared during the Miocene . Known fossil species include Tursiops osennae (late Miocene to early Pliocene ) from the Piacenzian coastal mudstone, and Tursiops miocaenus (Miocene) from the Burdigalian marine sandstone, all in Italy. The bottlenose dolphin weighs an average of 300 kg (660 lb), but can range from 150 and 650 kg (330 and 1,430 lb). It can reach

#296703