Misplaced Pages

Rotokas language

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.

Rotokas is a North Bougainville language spoken by about 4,320 people on the island of Bougainville , an island located to the east of New Guinea , which is part of Papua New Guinea . According to Allen and Hurd (1963), there are three identified dialects: Central Rotokas ("Rotokas Proper"), Aita Rotokas, and Pipipaia; with a further dialect spoken in Atsilima (Atsinima) village with an unclear status. Central Rotokas is most notable for its extremely small phonemic inventory and for having perhaps the smallest modern alphabet.

#360639

58-497: The Central dialect of Rotokas possesses one of the world's smallest phoneme inventories. (Only the Pirahã language has been claimed to have fewer.) The alphabet consists of twelve letters, representing eleven phonemes . Rotokas has a vowel length distinction (that is, all vowels have a short and long counterpart), but otherwise lacks distinctive suprasegmental features such as contrastive tone or stress. The consonant inventory embraces

116-487: A beautiful park is beautiful, but a car park is not "car". The modifier often indicates origin (" Virginia reel"), purpose (" work clothes"), semantic patient (" man eater") or semantic subject (" child actor"); however, it may generally indicate almost any semantic relationship. It is also common for adjectives to be derived from nouns, as in boyish , birdlike , behavioral (behavioural) , famous , manly , angelic , and so on. In Australian Aboriginal languages ,

174-447: A cause "), relative clauses (as in "the man who wasn't there "), and infinitive phrases (as in "a cake to die for "). Some nouns can also take complements such as content clauses (as in "the idea that I would do that "), but these are not commonly considered modifiers . For more information about possible modifiers and dependents of nouns, see Components of noun phrases . In many languages, attributive adjectives usually occur in

232-520: A consequence, observations involving concepts like the notion of quantity (which has a singular treatment in Pirahã language) became impossible, because of the influence of the new knowledge on the results. There is also a claim that Pirahã lacks any unique color terminology , being one of the few cultures (mostly in the Amazon basin and New Guinea) that only have specific words for 'light' and 'dark' if that claim

290-1073: A few loan words, mainly from Portuguese . Pirahã kóópo ("cup") is from the Portuguese word copo , and bikagogia ("business") comes from Portuguese mercadoria ("merchandise"). Everett (2005) says that the Pirahã culture has the simplest known kinship system of any human culture. A single word, baíxi (pronounced [màíʔì] ), is used for both 'mother' and 'father' (like English "parent" although Pirahã has no gendered alternative), and they appear not to keep track of relationships any more distant than biological siblings . According to Everett in 1986, Pirahã has words for 'one' ( hói ) and 'two' ( hoí ), distinguished only by tone. In his 2005 analysis, however, Everett said that Pirahã has no words for numerals at all, and that hói and hoí actually mean "small quantity" and "larger quantity". Frank et al. (2008) describes two experiments on four Pirahã speakers that were designed to test these two hypotheses. In one, ten spools of thread were placed on

348-420: A nominal element within a particular context. They generally do this by indicating definiteness ( a vs. the ), quantity ( one vs. some vs. many ), or another such property. An adjective acts as the head of an adjective phrase or adjectival phrase (AP). In the simplest case, an adjective phrase consists solely of the adjective; more complex adjective phrases may contain one or more adverbs modifying

406-635: A noun, they are far more circumscribed than adjectives in their use—typically, only a single determiner would appear before a noun or noun phrase (including any attributive adjectives). This means that, in English, adjectives pertaining to size precede adjectives pertaining to age ("little old", not "old little"), which in turn generally precede adjectives pertaining to colour ("old white", not "white old"). So, one would say "One (quantity) nice (opinion) little (size) old (age) round (shape) [ or round old] white (colour) brick (material) house." When several adjectives of

464-445: A paca there" Adjective An adjective ( abbreviated adj. ) is a word that describes or defines a noun or noun phrase . Its semantic role is to change information given by the noun. Traditionally, adjectives are considered one of the main parts of speech of the English language, although historically they were classed together with nouns . Nowadays, certain words that usually had been classified as adjectives, including

522-710: A phenomenon is odd cross-linguistically, Ian Maddieson has found in researching Pirahã data that /k/ does indeed exhibit an unusual distribution in the language. The "ten phoneme" claim also does not consider the tones of Pirahã, at least two of which are phonemic (marked by an acute accent and either unmarked or marked by a grave accent in Daniel Everett ), bringing the number of phonemes to at least twelve. Sheldon (1988) claims three tones, high (¹), mid (²) and low (³). When languages have inventories as small and allophonic variation as great as in Pirahã and Rotokas, different linguists may have very different ideas as to

580-417: A preposition "to", "for", etc. They may all be omitted, e.g., hi -ti -gi xai -bi i b-i ha i "he will send you to me". For possession, a pronoun is used in apposition ( zero-marking ): paitá Paita hi he xitóhoi testicles paitá hi xitóhoi Paita he testicles "Paita's testicles" ti I kaiíi house ti kaiíi I house "my house" Thomason & Everett (2001) note

638-401: A proper town (a real town, not a village) vs. They live in the town proper (in the town itself, not in the suburbs). All adjectives can follow nouns in certain constructions, such as tell me something new . In many languages, some adjectives are comparable and the measure of comparison is called degree . For example, a person may be "polite", but another person may be " more polite", and

SECTION 10

#1732843967361

696-688: A small closed class of adjectives, and new adjectives are not easily derived. Similarly, native Japanese adjectives ( i -adjectives) are considered a closed class (as are native verbs), although nouns (an open class) may be used in the genitive to convey some adjectival meanings, and there is also the separate open class of adjectival nouns ( na -adjectives). Many languages (including English) distinguish between adjectives, which qualify nouns and pronouns, and adverbs , which mainly modify verbs , adjectives, or other adverbs. Not all languages make this exact distinction; many (including English) have words that can function as either. For example, in English, fast

754-433: A special comparative form of the adjective. In such cases, as in some Australian Aboriginal languages , case-marking, such as the ablative case may be used to indicate one entity has more of an adjectival quality than (i.e. from —hence ABL) another. In English, many adjectives can be inflected to comparative and superlative forms by taking the suffixes "-er" and "-est" (sometimes requiring additional letters before

812-538: A specific order. In general, the adjective order in English can be summarised as: opinion, size, age or shape, colour, origin, material, purpose. Other language authorities, like the Cambridge Dictionary , state that shape precedes rather than follows age. Determiners and postdeterminers—articles, numerals, and other limiters (e.g. three blind mice)—come before attributive adjectives in English. Although certain combinations of determiners can appear before

870-414: A statement is only tentative or tendential: one might say "John is more the shy-and-retiring type", where the comparative "more" is not really comparing him with other people or with other impressions of him, but rather, could be substituting for "on the whole" or "more so than not". In Italian, superlatives are frequently used to put strong emphasis on an adjective: bellissimo means "most beautiful", but

928-403: A table one at a time and the Pirahã were asked how many were there. All four speakers answered in accordance with the hypothesis that the language has words for 'one' and 'two' in this experiment, uniformly using hói for one spool, hoí for two spools, and a mixture of the second word and 'many' for more than two spools. The second experiment, however, started with ten spools of thread on

986-486: A third person may be the " most polite" of the three. The word "more" here modifies the adjective "polite" to indicate a comparison is being made, and "most" modifies the adjective to indicate an absolute comparison (a superlative ). Among languages that allow adjectives to be compared, different means are used to indicate comparison. Some languages do not distinguish between comparative and superlative forms. Other languages allow adjectives to be compared but do not have

1044-459: A type of noun. The words that are today typically called nouns were then called substantive nouns ( nōmen substantīvum ). The terms noun substantive and noun adjective were formerly used in English but are now obsolete. Depending on the language, an adjective can precede a corresponding noun on a prepositive basis or it can follow a corresponding noun on a postpositive basis. Structural, contextual, and style considerations can impinge on

1102-554: Is a more recent innovation. There does not seem to be any reason for positing phonological manners of articulation (e.g., plosive, fricative, nasal, tap) in Central Rotokas. Rather, a simple binary distinction of voice is sufficient. Since a phonemic analysis is primarily concerned with distinctions, not with phonetic details, the symbols for voiced occlusives could be used: stop ⟨ b, d, ɡ ⟩ for Central Rotokas, and nasal ⟨ m, n, ŋ ⟩ for Aita dialect. (In

1160-595: Is a three-way distinction required between voiced, voiceless, and nasal consonants. Hence, this dialect has nine consonant phonemes versus six for Rotokas Proper (though no minimal pairs were found between /g/ and /ŋ/ ). The voiced and nasal consonants in Aita are collapsed in Central Rotokas, i.e., it is possible to predict the Central Rotokas form from the Aita Rotokas form, but not vice versa. For example, bokia ' day ' has /b ~ β/ in both Central and Aita Rotokas, but

1218-503: Is already absolute in its semantics. Such adjectives are called non-comparable or absolute . Nevertheless, native speakers will frequently play with the raised forms of adjectives of this sort. Although "pregnant" is logically non-comparable (either one is pregnant or not), one may hear a sentence like "She looks more and more pregnant each day". Comparative and superlative forms are also occasionally used for other purposes than comparison. In English comparatives can be used to suggest that

SECTION 20

#1732843967361

1276-519: Is an adjective in "a fast car" (where it qualifies the noun car ) but an adverb in "he drove fast " (where it modifies the verb drove ). In Dutch and German , adjectives and adverbs are usually identical in form and many grammarians do not make the distinction, but patterns of inflection can suggest a difference: A German word like klug ("clever(ly)") takes endings when used as an attributive adjective but not when used adverbially. Whether these are distinct parts of speech or distinct usages of

1334-503: Is being fronted . For example, the usual order of adjectives in English would result in the phrase "the bad big wolf" (opinion before size), but instead, the usual phrase is "the big bad wolf". Owing partially to borrowings from French, English has some adjectives that follow the noun as postmodifiers , called postpositive adjectives , as in time immemorial and attorney general . Adjectives may even change meaning depending on whether they precede or follow, as in proper : They live in

1392-420: Is called a "big house". Such an analysis is possible for the grammar of Standard Chinese and Korean , for example. Different languages do not use adjectives in exactly the same situations. For example, where English uses " to be hungry " ( hungry being an adjective), Dutch , French , and Spanish use " honger hebben ", " avoir faim ", and " tener hambre " respectively (literally "to have hunger",

1450-741: Is complicated by long vowels, and not all verbal conjugations follow this pattern. Typologically, Rotokas is a fairly typical verb-final language, with adjectives and demonstrative pronouns preceding the nouns they modify, and postpositions following. Although adverbs are fairly free in their ordering, they tend to precede the verb, as in the following example: osirei-toarei eye- MASC . DU avuka-va old- FEM . SG iava POST ururupa-vira closed- ADV tou-pa-si-veira be- PROG - 2 . DU . MASC - HAB osirei-toarei avuka-va iava ururupa-vira tou-pa-si-veira eye-MASC.DU old-FEM.SG POST closed-ADV be-PROG-2.DU.MASC-HAB The old woman's eyes are shut. The alphabet

1508-407: Is estimated to have between 250 and 380 speakers. It is not in immediate danger of extinction, as its use is vigorous and the Pirahã community is mostly monolingual. The Pirahã language is most notable as the subject of various controversial claims; for example, that it provides evidence against linguistic relativity . The controversy is compounded by the sheer difficulty of learning the language;

1566-462: Is in fact more commonly heard in the sense "extremely beautiful". Attributive adjectives and other noun modifiers may be used either restrictively (helping to identify the noun's referent, hence "restricting" its reference) or non-restrictively (helping to describe a noun). For example: Here "difficult" is restrictive – it tells which tasks he avoids, distinguishing these from the easy ones: "Only those tasks that are difficult". Here difficult

1624-485: Is non-restrictive – it is already known which task it was, but the adjective describes it more fully: "The aforementioned task, which (by the way) is difficult." In some languages, such as Spanish , restrictiveness is consistently marked; for example, in Spanish la tarea difícil means "the difficult task" in the sense of "the task that is difficult" (restrictive), whereas la difícil tarea means "the difficult task" in

1682-746: Is perhaps the smallest in use, with only 12 letters of ISO basic Latin alphabet without any diacritics and ligatures . The letters are A E G I K O P R S T U V . T and S both represent the phoneme /t/ , written with S before an I and in the name 'Rotokas', and with T elsewhere. The V is sometimes written B . A simpler alphabet has been proposed, using only A E I O U Ā Ē Ī Ō Ū P T K B D G , (16 letters) using macrons for long vowels and arguably simpler spelling rules. However, it has never been put into common use. Selected basic vocabulary items in Rotokas: Pirah%C3%A3 language Pirahã (also spelled Pirahá, Pirahán ), or Múra-Pirahã ,

1740-877: Is that some nominals seem to semantically denote entities (typically nouns in English) and some nominals seem to denote attributes (typically adjectives in English). Many languages have participle forms that can act as noun modifiers either alone or as the head of a phrase. Sometimes participles develop into functional usage as adjectives. Examples in English include relieved (the past participle of relieve ), used as an adjective in passive voice constructs such as "I am so relieved to see you". Other examples include spoken (the past participle of speak ) and going (the present participle of go ), which function as attribute adjectives in such phrases as "the spoken word" and "the going rate". Other constructs that often modify nouns include prepositional phrases (as in "a rebel without

1798-652: Is the indigenous language of the Pirahã people of Amazonas, Brazil . The Pirahã live along the Maici River , a tributary of the Amazon River . Pirahã is the only surviving dialect of the Mura language ; all others having died out in the last few centuries as most groups of the Mura people have shifted to Portuguese . Due to this, Pirahã can be considered its own language now, as no other Mura dialects have survived. Suspected relatives, such as Matanawi , are also extinct . Pirahã

Rotokas language - Misplaced Pages Continue

1856-762: Is true. Although the Pirahã glossary in Daniel Everett's Ph.D. thesis includes a list of color words (p. 354), Everett (2006) now says that the items listed in this glossary are not in fact words but descriptive phrases (such as "(like) blood" for "red"). The basic Pirahã personal pronouns are: These can be serially combined: ti gíxai or ti hi to mean "we" ( inclusive and exclusive ), and gíxai hi to mean "you (plural)", or combined with xogiáagaó 'all', as in "we (all) go". There are several other pronouns reported, such as 'she', 'it' (animal), 'it' (aquatic animal), and 'it' (inanimate), but these may actually be nouns, and they cannot be used independently

1914-510: The , this , my , etc., typically are classed separately, as determiners . Examples: Adjective comes from Latin nōmen adjectīvum , a calque of Ancient Greek : ἐπίθετον ὄνομα (surname) , romanized :  epítheton ónoma , lit.   'additional noun' (whence also English epithet ). In the grammatical tradition of Latin and Greek, because adjectives were inflected for gender, number, and case like nouns (a process called declension ), they were considered

1972-422: The Aita dialect before /i/ . Firchow & Firchow had reported the same for Central Rotokas, though Robinson contests it is not the case anymore due to widespread bilingualism with Tok Pisin . The voiced consonants are the allophonic sets [β, b, m] , [ɾ, n, l, d] , and [ɡ, ɣ, ŋ] . It is unusual for languages to lack phonemes whose primary allophone is a nasal . Firchow & Firchow (1969) have this to say on

2030-534: The Tupian pronouns were borrowed, and hi differs only in dropping the a . Pirahã is agglutinative , using a large number of affixes to communicate grammatical meaning. Even the 'to be' verbs of existence or equivalence are suffixes in Pirahã. For instance, the Pirahã sentence "there is a paca there" uses just two words; the copula is a suffix on "paca": káixihíxao-xaagá paca-exists gáihí there káixihíxao-xaagá gáihí paca-exists there "There's

2088-496: The adjective moorrooloo 'little' in the phrase moorrooloo baawa 'little child' can stand on its own to mean 'the little one,' while the attributive noun aamba 'man' in the phrase aamba baawa 'male child' cannot stand for the whole phrase to mean 'the male one.' In other languages, like Warlpiri , nouns and adjectives are lumped together beneath the nominal umbrella because of their shared syntactic distribution as arguments of predicates . The only thing distinguishing them

2146-512: The adjective (" very strong"), or one or more complements (such as "worth several dollars ", "full of toys ", or "eager to please "). In English, attributive adjective phrases that include complements typically follow the noun that they qualify ("an evildoer devoid of redeeming qualities "). In many languages (including English) it is possible for nouns to modify other nouns. Unlike adjectives, nouns acting as modifiers (called attributive nouns or noun adjuncts ) usually are not predicative;

2204-472: The distinction between adjectives and nouns is typically thought weak, and many of the languages only use nouns—or nouns with a limited set of adjective-deriving affix es—to modify other nouns. In languages that have a subtle adjective-noun distinction, one way to tell them apart is that a modifying adjective can come to stand in for an entire elided noun phrase, while a modifying noun cannot. For example, in Bardi ,

2262-510: The following places of articulation: bilabial , alveolar , and velar , each with a voiced and an unvoiced consonant. The three voiced members of the Central Rotokas dialect consonant phoneme inventory each have wide allophonic variation. Therefore, it is difficult to find a choice of IPA symbols to represent them which is not misleading. The voiceless consonants are straightforward voiceless stop consonants : /p, t, k/ [p, t, k] . Robinson (2006) reports that t has an allophone [ts]~[s] in

2320-400: The lack of nasal phonemes in the Central Rotokas dialect (which they call Rotokas Proper ): "In Rotokas Proper [...] nasals are rarely heard except when a native speaker is trying to imitate a foreigner’s attempt to speak Rotokas. In this case the nasals are used in the mimicry whether they were pronounced by the foreign speaker or not." Robinson shows that in the Aita dialect of Rotokas there

2378-502: The nature of their phonological systems. The segmental phonemes are: The number of phonemes is at most thirteen, matching Hawaiian , if [k] is counted as a phoneme and there are just two tones; if [k] is not phonemic, there are twelve phonemes, one more than the number found in Rotokas, or eleven among women who uniformly replace /s/ with /h/. ( English , by comparison, has thirty to forty-five , depending on dialect .) However, many of

Rotokas language - Misplaced Pages Continue

2436-653: The number of linguists with field experience in Pirahã is very small. Speakers refer to their language as Apáitisí , and to their own ethnic group as Hiáitihí . The Pirahã language is one of the phonologically simplest languages known, comparable to Rotokas ( New Guinea ) and the Lakes Plain languages such as Obokuitai . There is a claim that Pirahã has as few as ten phonemes , one fewer than Rotokas, or even as few as nine for women, but this requires analyzing [k] as an underlying /hi/ and having /h/ invariably substituted for /s/ in female speech. Although such

2494-409: The phonemes show a great deal of allophonic variation. For instance, vowels are nasalized after the glottal consonants /h/ and /ʔ/ (written h and x ). Also, Because of its variation, Everett states that /k/ is not a stable phoneme. By analyzing it as /hi/ , he is able to theoretically reduce the number of consonants to seven (or six for women with constant /h/-substitution). Pirahã has

2552-464: The phrase "a Ford car", "Ford" is unquestionably a noun but its function is adjectival: to modify "car". In some languages adjectives can function as nouns: for example, the Spanish phrase " un rojo " means "a red [one]". As for "confusion" with verbs, rather than an adjective meaning "big", a language might have a verb that means "to be big" and could then use an attributive verb construction analogous to "big-being house" to express what in English

2610-409: The pre-or post-position of an adjective in a given instance of its occurrence. In English, occurrences of adjectives generally can be classified into one of three categories: Adjectives feature as a part of speech (word class) in most languages . In some languages, the words that serve the semantic function of adjectives are categorized together with some other class, such as nouns or verbs . In

2668-476: The pronouns are formally close to those of the Tupian languages Nheengatu and Tenharim , which the Mura had once used as contact languages: Both the Tupian and Pirahã third-person pronouns can be used as demonstratives, as in Pirahã hi xobaaxai ti "I am really smart" ( lit. "This one sees well: me"). Given the restricted set of Pirahã phonemes, the Pirahã pronouns ti and gi are what one would expect if

2726-452: The proposed alphabet for Central Rotokas, these are written ⟨v, r, g⟩ . However, ⟨b, d, g⟩ would work equally well.) In the chart below, the most frequent allophones are used to represent the phonemes. Vowels may be long (written doubled) or short. It is uncertain whether these represent ten phonemes or five; that is, whether 'long' vowels are distinct speech sounds or mere sequences of two vowels that happen to be

2784-511: The same part of speech is a question of analysis. While German linguistic terminology distinguishes adverbiale from adjektivische Formen , German refers to both as Eigenschaftswörter ("property words"). Linguists today distinguish determiners from adjectives, considering them to be two separate parts of speech (or lexical categories ). Determiners formerly were considered to be adjectives in some of their uses. Determiners function neither as nouns nor pronouns but instead characterize

2842-516: The same type are used together, they are ordered from general to specific, like "lovely intelligent person" or "old medieval castle". This order may be more rigid in some languages than others; in some, like Spanish, it may only be a default ( unmarked ) word order, with other orders being permissible. Other languages, such as Tagalog , follow their adjectival orders as rigidly as English. The normal adjectival order of English may be overridden in certain circumstances, especially when one adjective

2900-413: The same. The Aita dialect appears not to distinguish length in vowels at all. Other vowel sequences are extremely common, as in the word upiapiepaiveira . It does not appear that stress is phonemic, but this is not certain. Words with 2 or 3 syllables are stressed on the initial syllable; those with 4 are stressed on the first and third; and those with 5 or more on the antepenultimate (third-last). This

2958-439: The second person plural pronoun in Central Rotokas starts with /b ~ β/ , /bisi/ , but with /m/ in its Aita cognate. Furthermore, Aita was found to have minimal pairs for the voiced labial and alveolar consonants: /buta/ ' time ' vs. /muta/ ' taste ' . This suggests that the consonant inventory of the ancestor language of Aita and Central Rotokas was more like Aita, and that the small phoneme inventory of Central Rotokas

SECTION 50

#1732843967361

3016-420: The sense of "the task, which is difficult" (non-restrictive). In English, restrictiveness is not marked on adjectives but is marked on relative clauses (the difference between "the man who recognized me was there" and "the man, who recognized me , was there" being one of restrictiveness). In some languages, adjectives alter their form to reflect the gender, case and number of the noun that they describe. This

3074-438: The suffix; see forms for far below), respectively: Some adjectives are irregular in this sense: Some adjectives can have both regular and irregular variations: also Another way to convey comparison is by incorporating the words "more" and "most". There is no simple rule to decide which means is correct for any given adjective, however. The general tendency is for simpler adjectives and those from Anglo-Saxon to take

3132-411: The suffixes, while longer adjectives and those from French , Latin , or Greek do not—but sometimes the sound of the word is the deciding factor. Many adjectives do not naturally lend themselves to comparison. For example, some English speakers would argue that it does not make sense to say that one thing is "more ultimate" than another, or that something is "most ultimate", since the word "ultimate"

3190-414: The table, and spools were subtracted one at a time. In this experiment, one speaker used hói (the word previously supposed to mean 'one') when there were six spools left, and all four speakers used that word consistently when there were as many as three spools left. Though Frank and his colleagues do not attempt to explain their subjects' difference in behavior in these two experiments, they conclude that

3248-464: The two words under investigation "are much more likely to be relative or comparative terms like 'few' or 'fewer' than absolute terms like 'one'". There is no grammatical distinction between singular and plural , even in pronouns. A 2012 documentary aired on the Smithsonian Channel reported that a school had been opened for the Pirahã community where they learn Portuguese and mathematics. As

3306-432: The way the three basic pronouns can. The fact that different linguists come up with different lists of such pronouns suggests that they are not basic to the grammar. In two recent papers, Everett cites Sheldon as agreeing with his (Everett's) analysis of the pronouns. Sheldon (1988) gives the following list of pronouns: Pronouns are prefixed to the verb, in the order SUBJECT-INDOBJECT-OBJECT where INDOBJECT includes

3364-419: The words for "hunger" being nouns). Similarly, where Hebrew uses the adjective זקוק ‎ ( zaqūq , roughly "in need of" or "needing"), English uses the verb "to need". In languages that have adjectives as a word class, it is usually an open class ; that is, it is relatively common for new adjectives to be formed via such processes as derivation . However, Bantu languages are well known for having only

#360639