Misplaced Pages

Proto-Munda language

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.

Proto-Munda is the reconstructed proto-language of the Munda languages of South Asia . It has been reconstructed by Sidwell & Rau (2015). According to Sidwell, the Proto-Munda language split from Proto- Austroasiatic in Indochina and arrived on the coast of Odisha around 4000 – 3500 years ago.

#609390

43-500: The following Proto-Munda lexical proto-forms have been reconstructed by Sidwell & Rau (2015: 319, 340-363). Two asterisks are given to denote the tentative, preliminary state of the proto-language reconstruction. Proto-Munda reconstruction has since been revised and improved by Rau (2019). Anderson (2011, 2017) proposes that syntax of the archaic stage of proto-Munda might have been possibly SVO or VOS. Evidence for SVO order has been founded in noun incorporation verbal morphology in

86-522: A consonant cluster or vowel sequence that is not permitted by the phonotactics of a language. Regular or semi-regular epenthesis commonly occurs in languages with affixes . For example, a reduced vowel /ɪ/ or /ə/ (here abbreviated as /ᵻ/ ) is inserted before the English plural suffix -/z/ and the past tense suffix -/d/ when the root ends in a similar consonant: glass → glasses /ˈɡlæsᵻz/ or /ˈɡlɑːsᵻz/ ; bat → batted /ˈbætᵻd/ . However, this

129-422: A diachronic analysis would see the original n disappearing except if a following vowel required its retention: an > a . However, a synchronic analysis, in keeping with the perception of most native speakers, would (though incorrectly) see it as epenthesis: a > an . In Dutch , whenever the suffix -er (which has several meanings) is attached to a word already ending in -r , an additional -d-

172-420: A stop consonant is often added as a transitional sound between the parts of a nasal + fricative sequence: The three short syllables in reliquiās do not fit into dactylic hexameter because of the dactyl 's limit of two short syllables so the first syllable is lengthened by adding another l . However, the pronunciation was often not written with double ll , and may have been the normal way of pronouncing

215-410: A bed.' In this example, the root for bed nakt has incorporated into the verbal construction and appears before the verbal root. Two other incidental changes are noticed here. First, the agreement marker in the first example is k and in the second example is ke . These are two phonologically-conditioned allomorphs. In other words, the choice between using k and ke is based on the other sounds in

258-729: A consonant in a medial cluster: - h j- . Some dialects, like Savo and Ostrobothnian , have epenthesis instead and use the preceding vowel in clusters of type -l C - and -h C - , in Savo also -nh- . (In Finnish linguistics, the phenomenon is often referred to as švaa ; the same word can also mean ' schwa ' , but it is not a phoneme in Finnish so there is usually no danger of confusion.) For example, Pohjanmaa ' Ostrobothnia ' → Pohojammaa , ryhmä → ryhymä , and Savo vanha → vanaha . Ambiguities may result: salmi ' strait ' vs. salami . (An exception

301-484: A default, epenthetic consonant in the intervocalic position". Epenthesis of a vowel is known as anaptyxis ( / ˌ æ n ə p ˈ t ɪ k s ɪ s / , from Greek ἀνάπτυξις ' unfolding ' ). Some accounts distinguish between "intrusive" optional vowels, vowel-like releases of consonants as phonetic detail, and true epenthetic vowels that are required by the phonotactics of the language and are acoustically identical with phonemic vowels. Many languages insert

344-412: A diachronic (historical) analysis, since epenthetic consonants are not used regularly in modern Japanese, the epenthetic /s/ could be from Old Japanese . It is also possible that Old Japanese /ame 2 / was once pronounced */same 2 /; the /s/ would then be not epenthetic but simply an archaic pronunciation. Another example is kosame ( 小雨 (こさめ) , 'light rain') . A complex example of epenthesis

387-500: A feature only of the spoken language. A consonant may be added to separate vowels in hiatus, as is the case with linking and intrusive R in English. A consonant may be placed between consonants in a consonant cluster where the place of articulation is different (such as if one consonant is labial and the other is alveolar ). A vowel may be placed between consonants to separate them. While epenthesis most often occurs between two vowels or two consonants, it can also occur between

430-423: A general statement. In this example: I bought a bed (and not a specific bed). In a broader sense, depending on context, it can even mean that I am a bed buyer, as in: I am a trader of beds, buying beds is my profession. waʼ- FACT - ke- 1 . SG - nakt- bed- a- EPEN - hninu- buy- ': PUNC waʼ- ke- nakt- a- hninu- ': FACT- 1.SG- bed- EPEN - buy- PUNC 'I bought

473-1066: A phonetic detail, it is usual to find a schwa vowel in sequences of a consonant followed by a flap. For instance, vinagre 'vinegar' may be [biˈnaɣɾe] but also [biˈnaɣᵊɾe] . Many Indo-Aryan languages carry an inherent vowel after each consonant. For example, in Assamese , the inherent vowel is "o" ( অ ), while in Hindi and Marathi , it is "a" ( अ ). Sanskrit words like maaŋsa ( ' meat ' , মাংস ), ratna ( ' jewel ' , ৰত্ন ), yatna ( ' effort ' , যত্ন ), padma ( ' lotus ' , পদ্ম ), harsha ( ' joy ' , হৰ্ষ ), dvaara ( ' door ' , দ্বাৰ ) etc. become moŋoh ( মাংস > মঙহ ), roton ( ৰত্ন > ৰতন ), zoton ( যত্ন > যতন ), podum ( পদ্ম > পদুম ), horix ( হৰ্ষ > হৰিষ ), duwar ( দ্বাৰ > দুৱাৰ ) etc. in Assamese. Other, non- Tatsama words also undergo anaptyxis, for example,

SECTION 10

#1732851284610

516-449: A separate cause: the pronunciation of nuclear as nucular ( /ˈn(j)ukjəlɚ/ ) in some North American dialects arises out of analogy with other - cular words ( binocular , particular , etc.) rather than from epenthesis. In colloquial registers of Brazilian Portuguese, [i] is sometimes inserted between consonant clusters except those with /l/ ( atleta ), /ɾ/ ( prato ) or syllable-ending /s/ ( pasta ; note syllable-final /s/

559-464: A so-called prop vowel at the end of a word, often as a result of the common sound change where vowels at the end of a word are deleted. For example, in the Gallo-Romance languages , a prop schwa /ə/ was added when final non-open vowels were dropped leaving /Cr/ clusters at the end, e.g. Latin nigrum '(shiny) black' > * [ˈnegro] > Old French negre /ˈnegrə/ 'black' (thus avoiding

602-471: A speaker finds a cluster difficult or impossible to pronounce. A vowel sound that is nonexistent in Lojban (usually /ɪ/ as in ' hit ' ) is added between two consonants to make the word easier to pronounce. Despite altering the phonetics of a word, the use of buffering is completely ignored by grammar. Also, the vowel sound used must not be confused with any existing Lojban vowel. An example of buffering in Lojban

645-465: A vowel and a consonant or at the ends of words. For example, the Japanese prefix ma- ( 真〜(ま〜) , 'pure …, complete …') transforms regularly to ma'- ( 真っ〜(まっ〜) , (gemination of following consonant)) when it is followed by a consonant, as in masshiro ( 真っ白(まっしろ) , 'pure white') . The English suffix -t , often found in the form -st , as in amongst (from among + -st ),

688-452: A word starting in rel- rather than a poetic modification. A limited number of words in Japanese use epenthetic consonants to separate vowels. An example is the word harusame ( 春雨 (はるさめ) , 'spring rain') , a compound of haru and ame in which an /s/ is added to separate the final /u/ of haru and the initial /a/ of ame . That is a synchronic analysis. As for

731-415: A word, especially in the beginning syllable ( prothesis ) or in the ending syllable ( paragoge ) or in-between two syllabic sounds in a word. The opposite process, where one or more sounds are removed, is referred to as elision . The word epenthesis comes from epi- ' in addition to ' and en- ' in ' and thesis ' putting ' . Epenthesis may be divided into two types: excrescence for

774-658: Is massao ( 真っ青 (まっさお) , 'deep blue, ghastly pale') , from ma- ( 真 〜(ま〜) , 'pure, complete') + ao ( 青 (あお) , 'blue') . It exhibits epenthesis on both morphemes: ma- ( 真〜(ま〜) ) → ma'- ( 真っ〜(まっ〜) , (gemination of following consonant)) is common (occurring before a consonant), and ao ( 青(あお) ) → sao ( 青(さお) ) occurs only in the example; it can be analyzed as maao → masao (intervocalic) → massao ; akin to kirisame ( 霧雨 (きりさめ) , 'drizzle, light rain') from kiri ( 霧 (きり) , 'fog, mist') + ame ( 雨 (あめ) , 'rain') . One hypothesis argues that Japanese /r/ developed "as

817-470: Is a synchronic analysis as the vowel was originally present in the suffix but has been lost in most words. Vocalic epenthesis typically occurs when words are borrowed from a language that has consonant clusters or syllable codas that are not permitted in the borrowing language. Languages use various vowels, but schwa is quite common when it is available: Epenthesis most often occurs within unfamiliar or complex consonant clusters. For example, in English,

860-600: Is a phenomenon by which a grammatical category , such as a verb , forms a compound with its direct object ( object incorporation ) or adverbial modifier, while retaining its original syntactic function. The inclusion of a noun qualifies the verb, narrowing its scope rather than making reference to a specific entity. Incorporation is central to many polysynthetic languages such as those found in North America, Siberia and northern Australia. However, polysynthesis does not necessarily imply incorporation (Mithun 2009), and

903-401: Is an example of terminal excrescence. Excrescence is the epenthesis of a consonant. In French , /t/ is inserted in inverted interrogative phrases between a verb ending in a vowel and a pronoun beginning with a vowel: il a ('he has') > a-t-il ('has he?'). There is no epenthesis from a historical perspective since the a-t is derived from Latin habet ('he has'), and so

SECTION 20

#1732851284610

946-440: Is inserted in between. For example, the comparative form of the adjective zoet ( ' sweet ' ) is zoeter , but the comparative of zuur ( ' sour ' ) is zuur d er and not the expected ** zurer . Similarly, the agent noun of verkopen ( ' to sell ' ) is verkoper ( ' salesperson ' ), but the agent noun of uitvoeren ( ' to perform ' ) is uitvoer d er ( ' performer ' ). In English,

989-475: Is pronounced [ʃ] in a number of dialects). Examples would be tsunami /tisuˈnami/ , advogado /adivoˈɡadu/ and abdômen [abiˈdomẽj] . Some dialects also use [e] , which is deemed as stereotypical of people from lower classes, such as those arriving from rural flight in internal migrations to cities such as Rio de Janeiro , Brasília and São Paulo . In Finnish , there are two epenthetic vowels and two nativization vowels. One epenthetic vowel

1032-433: Is that if a speaker finds the cluster [ml] in the word mlatu ( ' cat ' ) (pronounced ['mlatu] ) hard or impossible to pronounce, the vowel [ɐ] can be pronounced between the two consonants, resulting in the form [mɐˈlatu] . Nothing changes grammatically, including the word's spelling and the syllabication . A type of epenthesis in sign language is known as "movement epenthesis" and occurs, most commonly, during

1075-502: Is that in Pohjanmaa, -lj- and -rj- become -li- and -ri- , respectively: kirja → kiria . Also, in a small region in Savo, /e/ is used instead.) Lojban , a constructed language that seeks logically -oriented grammatical and phonological structures, uses a number of consonant clusters in its words. Since it is designed to be as universal as possible, it allows a type of anaptyxis called "buffering" to be used if

1118-528: Is the preceding vowel , found in the illative case ending -(h)*n : maa → maahan , talo → taloon . The second is [e] , connecting stems that have historically been consonant stems to their case endings: nim+n → nimen . In Standard Finnish, consonant clusters may not be broken by epenthetic vowels; foreign words undergo consonant deletion rather than addition of vowels: ranta ( ' shore ' ) from Proto-Germanic *strandō . However, modern loans may not end in consonants. Even if

1161-521: The t is the original third-person verb inflection. It is incorrect to call it epenthesis unless viewed synchronically since the modern basic form of the verb is a and so the psycholinguistic process is therefore the addition of t to the base form. A similar example is the English indefinite article a , which becomes an before a vowel. It originated from Old English ān ( ' one, a, an ' ), which retained an n in all positions, so

1204-502: The Sora - Gorum languages , and to a lesser extent in Gutob , Remo , Kharia , and Juang . Remo: gui-ti wash-hand gui-ti wash-hand "wash hand" Sora: ɲen 1SG dʒum-te-ti-n-ai eat-banana- NPST - 1 . INTR - 1 . SUBJ ɲen dʒum-te-ti-n-ai 1SG eat-banana-NPST-1.INTR-1.SUBJ "I am eating banana" Noun incorporation In linguistics , incorporation

1247-459: The closed syllable , resulting in городъ ( gorodŭ ), which became город ( gorod ) in modern Russian and Ukrainian. Other Slavic languages used metathesis for the vowel and the syllable-final consonant, producing * grodŭ in this case, as seen in Polish gród , Old Church Slavonic градъ gradŭ , Serbo-Croatian grad and Czech hrad . Another environment can be observed in

1290-634: The English word glass becomes gilas ( গিলাছ ). In the Western Romance languages , a prothetic vowel was inserted at the beginning of any word that began with /s/ and another consonant, e.g. Latin spatha 'two-edged sword, typically used by cavalry' becomes the normal word for 'sword' in Romance languages with an inserted /e/ : Spanish/Portuguese espada , Catalan espasa , Old French espede > modern épée (see also espadon ' swordfish '). French in fact presents three layers in

1333-488: The addition of a consonant , and for the addition of a vowel , svarabhakti (in Sanskrit) or alternatively anaptyxis ( / ˌ æ n ə p ˈ t ɪ k s ɪ s / ). Epenthesis arises for a variety of reasons. The phonotactics of a given language may discourage vowels in hiatus or consonant clusters , and a consonant or vowel may be added to make pronunciation easier. Epenthesis may be represented in writing, or it may be

Proto-Munda language - Misplaced Pages Continue

1376-430: The bed.' In this example, the verbal root hninu appears with its usual verbal morphology: a factive marker (FACT), which very roughly translates as past tense, although this is not quite accurate; an agreement marker (1.SG), which tells us that the verb agrees with 1st person singular (the speaker); and an aspect marker, punctual (PUNC), which tells us that this is a completed event. The direct object ne kanaktaʼ follows

1419-464: The history of Modern Persian , in which former word-initial consonant clusters, which were still extant in Middle Persian , are regularly broken up: Middle Persian brādar 'brother' > modern Iranian Persian برادر barādar /bærɑˈdær/ , Middle Persian stūn 'column' > Early New Persian ستون sutūn > modern Iranian Persian ستون sotun /soˈtun/ . In Spanish, as

1462-587: The impermissible /negr/ , cf. carrum > char 'cart'). Similarly as above, a vowel may be inserted in the middle of a word to resolve an impermissible word-final consonant cluster. An example of this can be found in Lebanese Arabic , where /ˈʔaləb/ 'heart' corresponds to Modern Standard Arabic قلب /qalb/ and Egyptian Arabic /ʔælb/ . In the development of Old English , Proto-Germanic *akraz 'field, acre' would have ended up with an impermissible /kr/ final cluster ( * æcr ), so it

1505-399: The incorporated form u' of "head", and the second its unincorporated form ipu : y-u'-kïti-ñe 3 -head-cut- IMPERF . TR amën 2SG y-u'-kïti-ñe amën 3-head-cut-IMPERF.TR 2SG "You head-cut it." y-ipu-n Epenthetic In phonology , epenthesis ( / ɪ ˈ p ɛ n θ ə s ɪ s , ɛ -/ ; Greek ἐπένθεσις ) means the addition of one or more sounds to

1548-436: The influence of Indo-Aryan languages like Hindi . Epenthesis is sometimes used for humorous or childlike effect. For example, the cartoon character Yogi Bear says "pic-a-nic basket" for picnic basket . Another example is found in the chants of England football fans in which England is usually rendered as [ˈɪŋɡələnd] or the pronunciation of athlete as "ath-e-lete". Some apparent occurrences of epenthesis, however, have

1591-492: The name Dwight is commonly pronounced with an epenthetic schwa between the /d/ and the /w/ ( [dəˈwaɪt] ), and many speakers insert a schwa between the /l/ and /t/ of realtor . Irish English and Scottish English are some of the dialects that may insert a schwa between /l/ and /m/ in words like film ( [ˈfɪləm] ) under the influence of Celtic languages , a phenomenon that also occurs in Indian English due to

1634-865: The presence of incorporation does not imply that the language is polysynthetic. Although incorporation does not occur regularly, English uses it sometimes: breastfeed , and direct object incorporation, as in babysit . Etymologically, such verbs in English are usually back-formations : the verbs breastfeed and babysit are formed from the adjective breast-fed and the noun babysitter respectively. Incorporation and plain compounding may be fuzzy categories: consider backstabbing , name-calling , axe murder . The following example from Oneida ( Iroquoian ) illustrates noun incorporation. waʼ- FACT - k- 1 . SG - hninu- buy- ': PUNC ne ne ka- PREF - nakt- bed- aʼ SUF waʼ- k- hninu- ': ne ka- nakt- aʼ FACT- 1.SG- buy- PUNC ne PREF - bed- SUF 'I bought

1677-410: The verb. The function of the particle ne is to determine the bed: in the example, I bought this specific bed. The word for bed consists of a root nakt plus a prefix and a suffix. The notion of the root is important here, but the properties of the prefix and suffix do not matter for this discussion. In the following sentence, the bed is unspecified. Unspecified nouns can be incorporated, thus creating

1720-571: The vocabulary in which initial vowel epenthesis is or is not applied, depending on the time a word came into the language: Similarly, at some point in the Proto-Armenian language and Classical Armenian , the prothetic vowel ե was placed at the beginning of the word before the sound ր , leading to words like երախ ( ' animal mouth ' , erax ) from Iranian rax ( ' animal mouth ' ), or երազ ( ' dream ' , eraz ) from Iranian raz ( ' mystery ' ). Epenthesis often breaks up

1763-471: The word (and has nothing to do with noun incorporation). Also, there is an epenthetic vowel a between the nominal and verbal roots. This vowel is inserted to break up an illegal consonant cluster (and also has nothing to do with noun incorporation). The next example, from Panare , illustrates the cross-linguistically common phenomenon that the incorporated form of a noun may be significantly different from its unincorporated form. The first sentence contains

Proto-Munda language - Misplaced Pages Continue

1806-419: The word, such as a personal name, is native, a paragogic vowel is needed to connect a consonantal case ending to the word. The vowel is /i/ : (Inter)net → netti , or in the case of personal name, Bush + -sta → Bushista ' about Bush ' ( elative case ). Finnish has moraic consonants: l , h and n are of interest. In Standard Finnish, they are slightly intensified before

1849-658: Was resolved by inserting an /e/ before the rhotic consonant : æcer (cf. the use of a syllabic consonant in Gothic akrs ). Vowel insertion in the middle of a word can be observed in the history of the Slavic languages , which had a preference for open syllables in medieval times. An example of this is the Proto-Slavic form * gordŭ 'town', in which the East Slavic languages inserted an epenthetic copy vowel to open

#609390