Misplaced Pages

Yawalapití language

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.

Yawalapiti (Jaulapiti) is an Arawakan language of Brazil . The Agavotaguerra (Agavotoqueng) reportedly spoke the same language. Speakers of the language live in a village along the edge of the river Tuatuari, a tributary of the Kuluene River , located in the southern part of the Xingu Indigenous Park ( Upper Xingu ), in the state of Mato Grosso .

#945054

21-413: Yawalapiti and Waurá , an Arawakan language belonging to the same subgroup, share a very similar phonemic inventory. The main segments are classified in the following table. There are no voiced plosives or affricates in the language. The palatal [c] appears to be an allophone of /k/ occurring before the front vowel /i/ , e.g. [puˈluka] "countryside" vs. [naˈciɾu] "my aunt". Also, the fricative /ʂ/

42-558: Is SVO. In genitive constructions, the possessor precedes the possessed item. The same is true for other modifiers, especially demonstratives and numerals, while adjectives are free to occur before or after their referent. Waur%C3%A1 language Waurá (Wauja) is an Arawakan language spoken in the Xingu Indigenous Park of Brazil by the Waujá people. It is "partially intelligible " with Mehináku . The entire population speaks

63-426: Is an example of a binary possessive class system in which a language distinguishes two kinds of possession (alienable and inalienable). The alienability distinction is the most common kind of binary possessive class system, but it is not the only one. Some languages have more than two possessive classes. In Papua New Guinea , for example, Anêm has at least 20 classes, and Amele has 32. Statistically, 15–20% of

84-663: Is because Proto-Arawak *e has evolved as /ɨ/ while, at the same time, Proto-Arawak *i and *ɨ have merged resulting into /i/ in Yawalapiti. Syllables in Yawalapiti can be of the type V, CV and, only in word-final position, (C)Vʔ. The stress may fall on the penultimate or on the last syllable of a word. As other Arawakan languages , Yawalapiti is agglutinative and makes use of affixes , especially suffixes , to convey basic grammatical relations. Nominal suffixes can be divided into two groups: classifiers and derivational suffixes. The main classifiers found in Yawalapiti refer to

105-408: Is important to note that they are just the most common types of inalienable nouns. Languages with an alienable/inalienable possession distinction differ in which classes fall under each type of possession. However, if a language has such a distinction, kinship roles or body parts (or both) make up some of the entities that are inalienably possessed. Also, languages may make different distinctions within

126-412: Is impossible to say that a particular relationship is an example of inalienable possession without specifying the languages for which that holds true. For example, neighbor may be an inalienable noun in one language but alienable in another. Additionally, in some languages, one entity can be both alienably possessed and inalienably possessed, and its type of possession is influenced by other properties of

147-412: Is in free variation with its voiced counterpart and /ʐ/ respectively, e.g. [iˈʂa ~ iˈʐa] "canoe". There are also some phonotactic constraints that dictate what types of consonants are allowed to appear in certain positions inside a word. For instance, the sounds /tʃ, l, ɾ/ cannot occur before the vowel /ɨ/ , and the latter two are restricted to the medial position. In a similar manner, the liquid /ʎ/

168-404: Is necessarily "someone's leg" even if it is severed from the body), kinship terms (such as mother ), and part-whole relations (such as top ). Many languages reflect the distinction but vary in how they mark inalienable possession. Cross-linguistically, inalienability correlates with many morphological , syntactic , and semantic properties. In general, the alienable–inalienable distinction

189-406: Is only observed in medial position and never before /a/ . As for the voiceless /r̥/ , it is the only rhotic segment that is allowed to appear in any position and before any vowel. The semivowel /w/ is found in initial, medial and final position, while /j/ does not occur in final position. The glottal stop is automatic in words beginning or ending in a vowel, i.e. a word like /u/ "water"

210-473: Is pronounced as [ˈʔuʔ] . The Yawalapiti language has both oral and nasal vowels, as shown below. Although nasal vowels mostly occur before or after nasal consonants, e.g. [ˈĩmi] " pequi oil" or [ˈmũnu] "termite", there are cases in which they are found in non-nasal environments, e.g. [hã ~ hĩ] (emphatic particle). Simple vowels can form various diphthongs , mainly /iu, ui, ia, ai, au, ua, ɨu, uɨ/ . Contrary to Waurá, Yawalapiti has no /e/ sound. This

231-454: The asterisk). French cannot use the inalienable possession construction for a relationship that is alienable. Al to.the tavolo, table qualcuno someone gli it. DAT ha has segato sawn tutte all le the gambe legs Al tavolo, qualcuno gli ha segato tutte le gambe to.the table someone it.DAT has sawn all the legs 'The table, someone has sawn off all its legs' *   La

SECTION 10

#1732852207946

252-399: The case of uku "arrow", which becomes n-uku-la "my arrow", p-uku-la "your arrow", in-uku-la "his/her arrow", etc. Independent pronouns of first and second person are listed in the table below. For the third person, Yawalapiti speakers use demonstrative pronouns, which are also marked for gender. From a typological point of view, the order of constituents in Yawalapiti

273-558: The categories on how many and which entities are treated as inalienable. Moreover, some languages allow the same noun to be either alienable or inalienable. Thus, trying to determine if a noun is alienable or inalienable based on its meaning or its affiliation to a specific noun category (for instance, body parts ) can be difficult. Although the relationships listed above are likely to be instances of inalienable possession, those that are ultimately classified as inalienable depend on conventions that are specific by language and culture. It

294-682: The correspondent prefix of the third person singular and the pluralizer suffix -pa . When nouns beginning with p , k , t , m , n , w and j are modified by the prefix of second person singular or plural, their initial sounds are subject to the following morphophonemic changes. Thus, for example, -kuʃu "head" becomes hi-tʃuʃu "your head" (but nu-kuʃu "my head"), -palaka "face" becomes hi-ɾalaka "your face" (but nu-palata "my face") and -jakanati "saliva" becomes hi-akanati "your saliva" (but nu-jakanati "my saliva"). Apart from possessive prefixes, alienable nouns also receive alienable possessive suffixes, as in

315-516: The language. This Arawakan languages -related article is a stub . You can help Misplaced Pages by expanding it . Alienability (grammar) In linguistics , inalienable possession ( abbreviated INAL ) is a type of possession in which a noun is obligatorily possessed by its possessor. Nouns or nominal affixes in an inalienable possession relationship cannot exist independently or be "alienated" from their possessor. Inalienable nouns include body parts (such as leg , which

336-412: The meaning of the original noun is not known. tsɨmɨ-r̥i tapir- DS .long tsɨmɨ-r̥i tapir-DS.long boa constrictor ina-pi ?- DS .pointed ina-pi ?-DS.pointed fish bone Nominal and verbal prefixes intervene in the formation of possessive phrases, and can indicate the subject or the object of a verb phrase. The set of possessive prefixes of Yawalapiti is very similar to those of

357-538: The other hand, has a less permanent association between the two entities. For instance, most objects may or may not be possessed. When such types of objects are possessed, the possession is alienable . Alienable possession is used generally for tangible items that one might cease to own at some point (such as my money ), but inalienable possession generally refers to a perpetual relationship that cannot be readily severed (such as my mother or my arm ). The table above outlines some common inalienable relationships, but it

378-464: The other languages of the Arawakan branch. In vowel initial nouns and verbs, the forms ni- and pi- are frequently used with roots beginning in u , while the prefix aw- appears systematically before a . In nouns and verbs beginning in consonant, the form ti- occurs only when the root starts with h (in all the other cases, hi- is used). The third person plural is actually a circumfix formed by

399-525: The sentence. Thus, whether a certain type of relationship is described as alienable or inalienable can be arbitrary. In that respect, alienability is similar to other types of noun classes such as grammatical gender . The examples below illustrate that the same phrase, the table's legs , is regarded as inalienable possession in Italian but alienable possession in French : (1b) is ungrammatical (as indicated by

420-533: The shape of an object or some other characteristic of it, like texture, length and position. These morphemes attach to adjectives when they refer to a noun that needs a classifier, as in the following example. kulata-ja hot- CL .liquid u. water kulata-ja u. hot-CL.liquid water Hot water. natʃa clothes autsa-lu new- CL .wrapping natʃa autsa-lu clothes new-CL.wrapping New clothes. Derivational suffixes are applied to primitive nouns to form new compound nouns. Sometimes, however,

441-414: The world's languages have obligatory possession . With inalienable possession, the two entities have a permanent association in which the possessed has little control over their possessor. For instance, body parts (under normal circumstances) do not change and cannot be removed from their possessor. The following real-world relationships often fall under inalienable possession: Alienable possession, on

SECTION 20

#1732852207946
#945054