Misplaced Pages

Gemination

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.

In phonetics and phonology , gemination ( / ˌ dʒ ɛ m ɪ ˈ n eɪ ʃ ən / ; from Latin geminatio 'doubling', itself from gemini 'twins'), or consonant lengthening , is an articulation of a consonant for a longer period of time than that of a singleton consonant. It is distinct from stress . Gemination is represented in many writing systems by a doubled letter and is often perceived as a doubling of the consonant. Some phonological theories use 'doubling' as a synonym for gemination, while others describe two distinct phenomena.

#531468

120-499: Consonant length is a distinctive feature in certain languages, such as Japanese . Other languages, such as Greek , do not have word-internal phonemic consonant geminates. Consonant gemination and vowel length are independent in languages like Arabic, Japanese, Finnish and Estonian; however, in languages like Italian, Norwegian , and Swedish , vowel length and consonant length are interdependent. For example, in Norwegian and Swedish,

240-492: A v after a u is geminated by most people: ruuvi 'screw' /ruːʋːi/ , vauva 'baby' [ʋauʋːa] . In the Tampere dialect, if a word receives gemination of v after u , the u is often deleted ( ruuvi [ruʋːi] , vauva [ʋaʋːa] ), and lauantai 'Saturday', for example, receives a medial v [lauʋantai] , which can in turn lead to deletion of u ( [laʋːantai] ). Distinctive consonant length

360-559: A pure vowel system, phonemic vowel and consonant length, and a lexically significant pitch-accent . Word order is normally subject–object–verb with particles marking the grammatical function of words, and sentence structure is topic–comment . Sentence-final particles are used to add emotional or emphatic impact, or form questions. Nouns have no grammatical number or gender , and there are no articles . Verbs are conjugated , primarily for tense and voice , but not person . Japanese adjectives are also conjugated. Japanese has

480-488: A 3-to-1 ratio, compared with around 2-to-1 (or lower) in Japanese, Italian, and Turkish. Gemination of consonants is distinctive in some languages and then is subject to various phonological constraints that depend on the language. In some languages, like Italian, Swedish, Faroese , Icelandic , and Luganda , consonant length and vowel length depend on each other. A short vowel within a stressed syllable almost always precedes

600-624: A benefit from the in-group to the out-group) means "[I/we] explained [it] to [him/her/them]". Such beneficiary auxiliary verbs thus serve a function comparable to that of pronouns and prepositions in Indo-European languages to indicate the actor and the recipient of an action. Japanese "pronouns" also function differently from most modern Indo-European pronouns (and more like nouns) in that they can take modifiers as any other noun may. For instance, one does not say in English: The amazed he ran down

720-570: A casa ('I am going home') [ˈvaːdo a kˈkaːsa] . All consonants except / z / can be geminated. This word-initial gemination is triggered either lexically by the item preceding the lengthening consonant (e.g. by preposition a 'to, at' in [a kˈkaːsa] a casa 'homeward' but not by definite article la in [la ˈkaːsa] la casa 'the house'), or by any word-final stressed vowel ([ parˈlɔ ffranˈtʃeːze ] parlò francese 's/he spoke French' but [ ˈparlo franˈtʃeːze ] parlo francese 'I speak French'). In Latin , consonant length

840-538: A complex system of honorifics , with verb forms and vocabulary to indicate the relative status of the speaker, the listener, and persons mentioned. The Japanese writing system combines Chinese characters , known as kanji ( 漢字 , ' Han characters') , with two unique syllabaries (or moraic scripts) derived by the Japanese from the more complex Chinese characters: hiragana ( ひらがな or 平仮名 , 'simple characters') and katakana ( カタカナ or 片仮名 , 'partial characters'). Latin script ( rōmaji ローマ字 )

960-468: A diacritic ( ḥaraka ) shaped like a lowercase Greek omega or a rounded Latin w , called the شَدَّة shadda : ّ  . Written above the consonant that is to be doubled, the shadda is often used to disambiguate words that differ only in the doubling of a consonant where the word intended is not clear from the context. For example, in Arabic, Form I verbs and Form II verbs differ only in

1080-414: A distinct language of its own that has absorbed various aspects from neighboring languages. Japanese has five vowels, and vowel length is phonemic, with each having both a short and a long version. Elongated vowels are usually denoted with a line over the vowel (a macron ) in rōmaji , a repeated vowel character in hiragana , or a chōonpu succeeding the vowel in katakana . /u/ ( listen )

1200-428: A four-way distinction among sibilant affricates /ts/ /tʂ/ /tʃ/ /tɕ/ , with one for each of the four tongue shapes. Toda also has a four-way sibilant distinction, with one alveolar, one palato-alveolar, and two retroflex (apical postalveolar and subapical palatal). The now-extinct Ubykh language was particularly complex, with a total of 27 sibilant consonants. Not only all four tongue shapes were represented (with

1320-410: A geminated consonant is always preceded by a short vowel, while an ungeminated consonant is preceded by a long vowel. Lengthened fricatives , nasals , laterals , approximants and trills are simply prolonged. In lengthened stops , the obstruction of the airway is prolonged, which delays release, and the "hold" is lengthened. In terms of consonant duration, Berber and Finnish are reported to have

SECTION 10

#1732885124532

1440-520: A generic "retracted sibilant" as [s̠] , a transcription frequently used for the sharper-quality types of retroflex consonants (e.g. the laminal "flat" type and the " apico-alveolar " type). There is no diacritic to denote the laminal "closed" articulation of palato-alveolars in the Northwest Caucasian languages , but they are sometimes provisionally transcribed as [ŝ ẑ] . The attested possibilities, with exemplar languages, are as follows. Note that

1560-419: A glide /j/ and either the first part of a geminate consonant ( っ / ッ , represented as Q) or a moraic nasal in the coda ( ん / ン , represented as N). The nasal is sensitive to its phonetic environment and assimilates to the following phoneme, with pronunciations including [ɴ, m, n, ɲ, ŋ, ɰ̃] . Onset-glide clusters only occur at the start of syllables but clusters across syllables are allowed as long as

1680-499: A grouping of sibilants and [f, v] , the term strident is more common. Some researchers judge [f] to be non-strident in English, based on measurements of its comparative amplitude, but to be strident in other languages (for example, in the African language Ewe , where it contrasts with non-strident [ɸ] ). The nature of sibilants as so-called 'obstacle fricatives' is complicated – there

1800-413: A higher pitched subset of the stridents. The English sibilants are: while the English stridents are: as /f/ and /v/ are stridents but not sibilants because they are lower in pitch. Be aware, some linguistics use the terms stridents and sibilants interchangeably to refer to the greater amplitude and pitch compared to other fricatives. "Stridency" refers to the perceptual intensity of

1920-479: A language. However, other possibilities exist. Serbo-Croatian has alveolar, flat postalveolar and alveolo-palatal affricates whereas Basque has palato-alveolar and laminal and apical alveolar ( apico-alveolar ) fricatives and affricates (late Medieval peninsular Spanish and Portuguese had the same distinctions among fricatives). Many languages, such as English or Arabic , have two sibilant types, one hissing and one hushing. A wide variety of languages across

2040-429: A listener depending on the listener's relative social position and the degree of familiarity between the speaker and the listener. When used in different social relationships, the same word may have positive (intimate or respectful) or negative (distant or disrespectful) connotations. Japanese often use titles of the person referred to where pronouns would be used in English. For example, when speaking to one's teacher, it

2160-491: A long consonant or a consonant cluster, and a long vowel must be followed by a short consonant. In Classical Arabic , a long vowel was lengthened even more before permanently-geminate consonants . In other languages, such as Finnish , consonant length and vowel length are independent of each other. In Finnish, both are phonemic; taka /taka/ 'back', takka /takːa/ 'fireplace' and taakka /taːkːa/ 'burden' are different, unrelated words. Finnish consonant length

2280-697: A long consonant to a short one, which is called degemination . It is a pattern in Baltic-Finnic consonant gradation that the strong grade (often the nominative ) form of the word is degeminated into a weak grade (often all the other cases) form of the word: taakka > taakan (burden, of the burden). As a historical restructuring at the phonemic level , word-internal long consonants degeminated in Western Romance languages: e.g. Spanish /ˈboka/ 'mouth' vs. Italian /ˈbokka/, both of which evolved from Latin /ˈbukka/. Written Arabic indicates gemination with

2400-408: A sentence need not be stated and pronouns may be omitted if they can be inferred from context. In the example above, hana ga nagai would mean "[their] noses are long", while nagai by itself would mean "[they] are long." A single verb can be a complete sentence: Yatta! ( やった! ) "[I / we / they / etc] did [it]!". In addition, since adjectives can form the predicate in a Japanese sentence (below),

2520-428: A single adjective can be a complete sentence: Urayamashii! ( 羨ましい! ) "[I'm] jealous [about it]!". While the language has some words that are typically translated as pronouns, these are not used as frequently as pronouns in some Indo-European languages, and function differently. In some cases, Japanese relies on special verb forms and auxiliary verbs to indicate the direction of benefit of an action: "down" to indicate

SECTION 20

#1732885124532

2640-634: A stream of air with the tongue towards the teeth . Examples of sibilants are the consonants at the beginning of the English words sip , zip , ship , and genre . The symbols in the International Phonetic Alphabet used to denote the sibilant sounds in these words are, respectively, [s] [z] [ʃ] [ʒ] . Sibilants have a characteristically intense sound, which accounts for their paralinguistic use in getting one's attention (e.g. calling someone using "psst!" or quieting someone using "shhhh!"). In

2760-548: A subapical palatal retroflex sibilant occurs in Toda . The main distinction is the shape of the tongue. Most sibilants have a groove running down the centerline of the tongue that helps focus the airstream, but it is not known how widespread this is. In addition, the following tongue shapes are described, from sharpest and highest-pitched to dullest and lowest-pitched: The latter three post-alveolar types of sounds are often known as "hushing" sounds because of their quality, as opposed to

2880-624: Is compressed rather than protruded , or simply unrounded. Some Japanese consonants have several allophones , which may give the impression of a larger inventory of sounds. However, some of these allophones have since become phonemic. For example, in the Japanese language up to and including the first half of the 20th century, the phonemic sequence /ti/ was palatalized and realized phonetically as [tɕi] , approximately chi ( listen ) ; however, now [ti] and [tɕi] are distinct, as evidenced by words like tī [tiː] "Western-style tea" and chii [tɕii] "social status". The "r" of

3000-487: Is also affected by consonant gradation . Another important phenomenon is sandhi , which produces long consonants at word boundaries when there is an archiphonemic glottal stop |otaʔ se| > otas se 'take it ( imperative )!'. In addition, in some Finnish compound words, if the initial word ends in an e , the initial consonant of the following word is geminated: jätesäkki 'trash bag' [jætesːækːi] , tervetuloa 'welcome' [terʋetːuloa] . In certain cases,

3120-445: Is also seen in o-medetō "congratulations", from medetaku ). Late Middle Japanese has the first loanwords from European languages – now-common words borrowed into Japanese in this period include pan ("bread") and tabako ("tobacco", now "cigarette"), both from Portuguese . Modern Japanese is considered to begin with the Edo period (which spanned from 1603 to 1867). Since Old Japanese,

3240-519: Is also used in a limited fashion (such as for imported acronyms) in Japanese writing. The numeral system uses mostly Arabic numerals , but also traditional Chinese numerals . Proto-Japonic , the common ancestor of the Japanese and Ryukyuan languages , is thought to have been brought to Japan by settlers coming from the Korean peninsula sometime in the early- to mid-4th century BC (the Yayoi period ), replacing

3360-440: Is appropriate to use sensei ( 先生 , "teacher"), but inappropriate to use anata . This is because anata is used to refer to people of equal or lower status, and one's teacher has higher status. Japanese nouns have no grammatical number, gender or article aspect. The noun hon ( 本 ) may refer to a single book or several books; hito ( 人 ) can mean "person" or "people", and ki ( 木 ) can be "tree" or "trees". Where number

3480-684: Is associated with comedy (see Kansai dialect ). Dialects of Tōhoku and North Kantō are associated with typical farmers. The Ryūkyūan languages, spoken in Okinawa and the Amami Islands (administratively part of Kagoshima ), are distinct enough to be considered a separate branch of the Japonic family; not only is each language unintelligible to Japanese speakers, but most are unintelligible to those who speak other Ryūkyūan languages. However, in contrast to linguists, many ordinary Japanese people tend to consider

3600-462: Is better documentation of Late Middle Japanese phonology than for previous forms (for instance, the Arte da Lingoa de Iapam ). Among other sound changes, the sequence /au/ merges to /ɔː/ , in contrast with /oː/ ; /p/ is reintroduced from Chinese; and /we/ merges with /je/ . Some forms rather more familiar to Modern Japanese speakers begin to appear – the continuative ending - te begins to reduce onto

3720-509: Is correlated with the sex of the speaker and the social situation in which they are spoken: men and women alike in a formal situation generally refer to themselves as watashi ( 私 , literally "private") or watakushi (also 私 , hyper-polite form), while men in rougher or intimate conversation are much more likely to use the word ore ( 俺 "oneself", "myself") or boku . Similarly, different words such as anata , kimi , and omae ( お前 , more formally 御前 "the one before me") may refer to

Gemination - Misplaced Pages Continue

3840-588: Is distinctive (as is vowel length). Gemination in the syllabary is represented with the sokuon , a small tsu : っ for hiragana in native words and ッ for katakana in foreign words. For example, 来た ( きた , kita ) means 'came; arrived', while 切った ( きった , kitta ) means 'cut; sliced'. With the influx of gairaigo ('foreign words') into Modern Japanese, voiced consonants have become able to geminate as well: バグ ( bagu ) means '(computer) bug', and バッグ ( baggu ) means 'bag'. Distinction between voiceless gemination and voiced gemination

3960-609: Is distinctive in Punjabi, for example: In Russian , consonant length (indicated with two letters, as in ва нн а [ˈva nn ə] 'bathtub') may occur in several situations. Minimal pairs (or chronemes ) exist, such as по д ержать [pə d ʲɪrˈʐatʲ] 'to hold' vs по дд ержать [pə dʲː ɪrˈʐatʲ] 'to support', and their conjugations, or дли н а [dlʲɪˈ n a] 'length' vs дли нн а [dlʲɪˈ nː a] 'long' adj. f. There are phonetic geminate consonants in Caribbean Spanish due to

4080-417: Is important, it can be indicated by providing a quantity (often with a counter word ) or (rarely) by adding a suffix, or sometimes by duplication (e.g. 人人 , hitobito , usually written with an iteration mark as 人々 ). Words for people are usually understood as singular. Thus Tanaka-san usually means Mx Tanaka . Words that refer to people and animals can be made to indicate a group of individuals through

4200-503: Is indicated by two identical letters as in most languages that have phonemic gemination. Japanese language Japanese ( 日本語 , Nihongo , [ɲihoŋɡo] ) is the principal language of the Japonic language family spoken by the Japanese people . It has around 123 million speakers, primarily in Japan , the only country where it is the national language , and within

4320-473: Is indicated with two identical letters. Examples: Consonant length is distinctive and sometimes is necessary to distinguish words: Double consonants are common on morpheme borders where the initial or final sound of the suffix is the same as the final or initial sound of the stem (depending on the position of the suffix), after devoicing . Examples: Punjabi is written in two scripts, namely, Gurmukhi and Shahmukhi . Both scripts indicate gemination through

4440-722: Is less common. In terms of mutual intelligibility , a survey in 1967 found that the four most unintelligible dialects (excluding Ryūkyūan languages and Tōhoku dialects ) to students from Greater Tokyo were the Kiso dialect (in the deep mountains of Nagano Prefecture ), the Himi dialect (in Toyama Prefecture ), the Kagoshima dialect and the Maniwa dialect (in Okayama Prefecture ). The survey

4560-813: Is lexically contrastive. The distinction between single and geminate consonants is attested in medial position as well as in absolute initial and final positions. In addition to lexical geminates, Berber also has phonologically-derived and morphologically-derived geminates. Phonological alternations can surface by concatenation (e.g., [fas sin] 'give him two!') or by complete assimilation (e.g. /rad = k i-sli/ [rakk isli] 'he will touch you'). Morphological alternations include imperfective gemination, with some Berber verbs forming their imperfective stem by geminating one consonant in their perfective stem (e.g., [ftu] 'go! PF', [fttu] 'go! IMPF'), as well as quantity alternations between singular and plural forms (e.g., [afus] 'hand', [ifassn] 'hands'). Austronesian languages in

4680-537: Is no longer distinctive. In Nepali , all consonants have geminate counterparts except for /w, j, ɦ/ . Geminates occur only medially. Examples: In Norwegian , gemination is indicated in writing by double consonants. Gemination often differentiates between unrelated words. As in Italian, Norwegian uses short vowels before doubled consonants and long vowels before single consonants. There are qualitative differences between short and long vowels: In Polish , consonant length

4800-430: Is not an IPA notation. See the article on postalveolar consonants for more information. The following table shows the types of sibilant fricatives defined in the International Phonetic Alphabet : Diacritics can be used for finer detail. For example, apical and laminal alveolars can be specified as [s̺] vs [s̻] ; a dental (or more likely denti-alveolar ) sibilant as [s̪] ; a palatalized alveolar as [sʲ] ; and

4920-466: Is not distinctive within root words . For instance, baggage is pronounced / ˈ b æ ɡ ɪ dʒ / , not */bæɡːɪdʒ/ . However, phonetic gemination does occur marginally. Gemination is found across words and across morphemes when the last consonant in a given word and the first consonant in the following word are the same fricative , nasal , or stop . For instance: With affricates , however, this does not occur. For instance: In most instances,

Gemination - Misplaced Pages Continue

5040-420: Is often called a topic-prominent language , which means it has a strong tendency to indicate the topic separately from the subject, and that the two do not always coincide. The sentence Zō wa hana ga nagai ( 象は鼻が長い ) literally means, "As for elephant(s), (the) nose(s) (is/are) long". The topic is zō "elephant", and the subject is hana "nose". Japanese grammar tends toward brevity; the subject or object of

5160-432: Is particularly important for retroflex sibilants, because all three varieties can occur, with noticeably different sound qualities. For more information on these variants and their relation to sibilants, see the article on postalveolar consonants . For tongue-down laminal articulations, an additional distinction can be made depending on where exactly behind the lower teeth the tongue tip is placed. A little ways back from

5280-746: Is phonemically /ˈbevve/ and pronounced [ˈbevːe] , while beve ('he/she drinks/is drinking') is /ˈbeve/ , pronounced [ˈbeːve] . Tonic syllables are bimoraic and are therefore composed of either a long vowel in an open syllable (as in beve ) or a short vowel in a closed syllable (as in bevve ). In varieties with post-vocalic weakening of some consonants (e.g. /raˈdʒone/ → [raˈʒoːne] 'reason'), geminates are not affected ( /ˈmaddʒo/ → [ˈmad͡ʒːo] 'May'). Double or long consonants occur not only within words but also at word boundaries, and they are then pronounced but not necessarily written: chi + sa = chissà ('who knows') [kisˈsa] and vado

5400-489: Is preserved in words such as matsuge ("eyelash", lit. "hair of the eye"); modern mieru ("to be visible") and kikoeru ("to be audible") retain a mediopassive suffix - yu(ru) ( kikoyu → kikoyuru (the attributive form, which slowly replaced the plain form starting in the late Heian period) → kikoeru (all verbs with the shimo-nidan conjugation pattern underwent this same shift in Early Modern Japanese )); and

5520-545: Is represented by the Shadda diacritic, which is usually omitted from writings, and mainly written to clear ambiguity. In Hindi, gemination is represented by doubling the geminated consonant, enjoined with the Virama diacritic. Gemination of aspirated consonants in Hindi are formed by combining the corresponding non-aspirated consonant followed by its aspirated counterpart. In vocalised Urdu,

5640-402: Is the version of Japanese discussed in this article. Formerly, standard Japanese in writing ( 文語 , bungo , "literary language") was different from colloquial language ( 口語 , kōgo ) . The two systems have different rules of grammar and some variance in vocabulary. Bungo was the main method of writing Japanese until about 1900; since then kōgo gradually extended its influence and

5760-471: Is used for the present and the future. For verbs that represent an ongoing process, the -te iru form indicates a continuous (or progressive) aspect , similar to the suffix ing in English. For others that represent a change of state, the -te iru form indicates a perfect aspect. For example, kite iru means "They have come (and are still here)", but tabete iru means "They are eating". Questions (both with an interrogative pronoun and yes/no questions) have

5880-437: Is usually restricted to certain consonants and environments. There are very few languages that have initial consonant length; among those that do are Pattani Malay , Chuukese , Moroccan Arabic , a few Romance languages such as Sicilian and Neapolitan , as well as many High Alemannic German dialects, such as that of Thurgovia . Some African languages, such as Setswana and Luganda , also have initial consonant length: it

6000-541: Is very common in Luganda and indicates certain grammatical features. In colloquial Finnish and Italian , long consonants occur in specific instances as sandhi phenomena. The difference between singleton and geminate consonants varies within and across languages. Sonorants show more distinct geminate-to-singleton ratios while sibilants have less distinct ratios. The bilabial and alveolar geminates are generally longer than velar ones. The reverse of gemination reduces

6120-544: Is visible in pairs of words such as キット ( kitto , meaning 'kit') and キッド ( kiddo , meaning 'kid'). In addition, in some variants of colloquial Modern Japanese, gemination may be applied to some adjectives and adverbs (regardless of voicing) in order to add emphasis: すごい ( sugoi , 'amazing') contrasts with すっごい ( suggoi , ' really amazing'); 思い切り ( おもいきり , omoikiri , 'with all one's strength') contrasts with 思いっ切り ( おもいっきり , omoikkiri , ' really with all one's strength'). In Turkish gemination

SECTION 50

#1732885124532

6240-405: Is why some linguists do not classify Japanese "pronouns" as pronouns, but rather as referential nouns, much like Spanish usted (contracted from vuestra merced , "your ( majestic plural ) grace") or Portuguese você (from vossa mercê ). Japanese personal pronouns are generally used only in situations requiring special emphasis as to who is doing what to whom. The choice of words used as pronouns

6360-457: Is written above the geminated consonant. In the cases of aspirated consonants in the Shahmukhi script , the shadda remains on the consonant, not on the do-cashmī he . Gemination is specially characteristic of Punjabi compared to other Indo-Aryan languages like Hindi-Urdu, where instead of the presence of consonant lengthening, the preceding vowel tends to be lengthened. Consonant length

6480-676: The Japanese diaspora worldwide. The Japonic family also includes the Ryukyuan languages and the variously classified Hachijō language . There have been many attempts to group the Japonic languages with other families such as the Ainu , Austronesian , Koreanic , and the now-discredited Altaic , but none of these proposals have gained any widespread acceptance. Little is known of the language's prehistory, or when it first appeared in Japan. Chinese documents from

6600-455: The Japonic language family, which also includes the Ryukyuan languages spoken in the Ryukyu Islands . As these closely related languages are commonly treated as dialects of the same language, Japanese is sometimes called a language isolate . According to Martine Irma Robbeets , Japanese has been subject to more attempts to show its relation to other languages than any other language in

6720-738: The Philippines , Micronesia , and Sulawesi are known to have geminate consonants. The Formosan language Kavalan makes use of gemination to mark intensity, as in sukaw 'bad' vs. sukkaw 'very bad'. Word-initial gemination occurs in various Malay dialects, particularly those found on the east coast of the Malay Peninsula such as Kelantan-Pattani Malay and Terengganu Malay . Gemination in these dialects of Malay occurs for various purposes such as: The Polynesian language Tuvaluan allows for word-initial geminates, such as mmala 'overcooked'. In English phonology , consonant length

6840-451: The Philippines , and various Pacific islands, locals in those countries learned Japanese as the language of the empire. As a result, many elderly people in these countries can still speak Japanese. Japanese emigrant communities (the largest of which are to be found in Brazil , with 1.4 million to 1.5 million Japanese immigrants and descendants, according to Brazilian IBGE data, more than

6960-716: The United States (notably in Hawaii , where 16.7% of the population has Japanese ancestry, and California ), and the Philippines (particularly in Davao Region and the Province of Laguna ). Japanese has no official status in Japan, but is the de facto national language of the country. There is a form of the language considered standard : hyōjungo ( 標準語 ) , meaning "standard Japanese", or kyōtsūgo ( 共通語 ) , "common language", or even "Tokyo dialect" at times. The meanings of

7080-470: The alveolar hissing sibilants [s] and [z] , the back of the tongue forms a narrow channel (is grooved ) to focus the stream of air more intensely, resulting in a high pitch. With the hushing sibilants (occasionally termed shibilants ), such as English [ʃ] , [tʃ] , [ʒ] , and [dʒ] , the tongue is flatter, and the resulting pitch lower. A broader category is stridents , which include more fricatives than sibilants such as uvulars . Sibilants are

7200-794: The de facto standard Japanese had been the Kansai dialect , especially that of Kyoto . However, during the Edo period, Edo (now Tokyo) developed into the largest city in Japan, and the Edo-area dialect became standard Japanese. Since the end of Japan's self-imposed isolation in 1853, the flow of loanwords from European languages has increased significantly. The period since 1945 has seen many words borrowed from other languages—such as German, Portuguese and English. Many English loan words especially relate to technology—for example, pasokon (short for "personal computer"), intānetto ("internet"), and kamera ("camera"). Due to

7320-545: The shadda is placed on the unaspirated consonant followed by the short vowel diacritic , followed by the do-cashmī hē , which aspirates the preceding consonant. There are few examples where an aspirated consonant is truly doubled. Italian is notable among the Romance languages for its extensive geminated consonants. In Standard Italian , word-internal geminates are usually written with two consonants, and geminates are distinctive. For example, bevve , meaning 'he/she drank',

SECTION 60

#1732885124532

7440-474: The standard and most other varieties , with the exception of Cypriot (where it might carry over from Ancient Greek or arise from a number of synchronic and diachronic assimilatory processes, or even spontaneously), some varieties of the southeastern Aegean, and Italy . Gemination is common in both Hindi and Urdu . It does not occur after long vowels and is found in words of both Indic and Arabic origin, but not in those of Persian origin. In Urdu, gemination

7560-448: The standard dialect moved from the Kansai region to the Edo region (modern Tokyo ) in the Early Modern Japanese period (early 17th century–mid 19th century). Following the end of Japan's self-imposed isolation in 1853, the flow of loanwords from European languages increased significantly, and words from English roots have proliferated. Japanese is an agglutinative , mora -timed language with relatively simple phonotactics ,

7680-480: The " ceceo " type, which have replaced the former hissing fricative with [θ] , leaving only [tʃ] . Languages with no sibilants are fairly rare. Most have no fricatives at all or only the fricative /h/ . Examples include most Australian languages , and Rotokas , and what is generally reconstructed for Proto-Bantu . Languages with fricatives but no sibilants, however, do occur, such as Ukue in Nigeria , which has only

7800-412: The "hissing" alveolar sounds. The alveolar sounds in fact occur in several varieties, in addition to the normal sound of English s : Speaking non-technically, the retroflex consonant [ʂ] sounds somewhat like a mixture between the regular English [ʃ] of "ship" and a strong American "r"; while the alveolo-palatal consonant [ɕ] sounds somewhat like a mixture of English [ʃ] of "ship" and the [sj] in

7920-518: The 1.2 million of the United States ) sometimes employ Japanese as their primary language. Approximately 12% of Hawaii residents speak Japanese, with an estimated 12.6% of the population of Japanese ancestry in 2008. Japanese emigrants can also be found in Peru , Argentina , Australia (especially in the eastern states), Canada (especially in Vancouver , where 1.4% of the population has Japanese ancestry),

8040-461: The 3rd century AD recorded a few Japanese words, but substantial Old Japanese texts did not appear until the 8th century. From the Heian period (794–1185), extensive waves of Sino-Japanese vocabulary entered the language, affecting the phonology of Early Middle Japanese . Late Middle Japanese (1185–1600) saw extensive grammatical changes and the first appearance of European loanwords . The basis of

8160-465: The IPA diacritics are simplified; some articulations would require two diacritics to be fully specified, but only one is used in order to keep the results legible without the need for OpenType IPA fonts. Also, Ladefoged has resurrected an obsolete IPA symbol, the under dot, to indicate apical postalveolar (normally included in the category of retroflex consonants ), and that notation is used here. (Note that

8280-482: The Japanese language is of particular interest, ranging between an apical central tap and a lateral approximant . The "g" is also notable; unless it starts a sentence, it may be pronounced [ ŋ ] , in the Kanto prestige dialect and in other eastern dialects. The phonotactics of Japanese are relatively simple. The syllable structure is (C)(G)V(C), that is, a core vowel surrounded by an optional onset consonant,

8400-724: The Old Japanese sections are written in Man'yōgana , which uses kanji for their phonetic as well as semantic values. Based on the Man'yōgana system, Old Japanese can be reconstructed as having 88 distinct morae . Texts written with Man'yōgana use two different sets of kanji for each of the morae now pronounced き (ki), ひ (hi), み (mi), け (ke), へ (he), め (me), こ (ko), そ (so), と (to), の (no), も (mo), よ (yo) and ろ (ro). (The Kojiki has 88, but all later texts have 87. The distinction between mo 1 and mo 2 apparently

8520-488: The Ryūkyūan languages as dialects of Japanese. The imperial court also seems to have spoken an unusual variant of the Japanese of the time, most likely the spoken form of Classical Japanese , a writing style that was prevalent during the Heian period , but began to decline during the late Meiji period . The Ryūkyūan languages are classified by UNESCO as 'endangered', as young people mostly use Japanese and cannot understand

8640-431: The absence of this doubling does not affect the meaning, though it may confuse the listener momentarily. The following minimal pairs represent examples where the doubling does affect the meaning in most accents: Note that whenever [(ɹ)] appears (in brackets), non-rhotic dialects of English don't have the gemination, but rather lengthen the preceding vowel. In some dialects gemination is also found for some words when

8760-543: The addition of a collective suffix (a noun suffix that indicates a group), such as -tachi , but this is not a true plural: the meaning is closer to the English phrase "and company". A group described as Tanaka-san-tachi may include people not named Tanaka. Some Japanese nouns are effectively plural, such as hitobito "people" and wareware "we/us", while the word tomodachi "friend" is considered singular, although plural in form. Verbs are conjugated to show tenses, of which there are two: past and present (or non-past) which

8880-625: The assimilation of /l/ and /ɾ/ in syllabic coda to the following consonant. Examples of Cuban Spanish: Luganda (a Bantu language ) is unusual in that gemination can occur word-initially, as well as word-medially. For example, kkapa /kːapa/ 'cat', /ɟːaɟːa/ jjajja 'grandfather' and /ɲːabo/ nnyabo 'madam' all begin with geminate consonants. There are three consonants that cannot be geminated: /j/ , /w/ and /l/ . Whenever morphological rules would geminate these consonants, /j/ and /w/ are prefixed with /ɡ/ , and /l/ changes to /d/ . For example: In Japanese , consonant length

9000-441: The conditional (and possibly the future tense) from the imperfect: courrais 'would run' /kuʁ.ʁɛ/ vs. courais 'ran' /ku.ʁɛ/ ; or the indicative from the subjunctive: croyons 'we believe' /kʁwa.jɔ̃/ vs. croyions 'we believed' /kʁwaj.jɔ̃/ . In Ancient Greek , consonant length was distinctive, e.g., μέ λ ω [mélɔː] 'I am of interest' vs. μέ λλ ω [mélːɔː] 'I am going to'. The distinction has been lost in

9120-417: The doubling of the middle consonant of the triliteral root in the latter form, e. g. , درس darasa (with full diacritics: دَرَسَ ) is a Form I verb meaning to study , whereas درّس darrasa (with full diacritics: دَرَّسَ ) is the corresponding Form II verb, with the middle r consonant doubled, meaning to teach . In Berber , each consonant has a geminate counterpart, and gemination

9240-564: The effect of changing Japanese into a mora-timed language. Late Middle Japanese covers the years from 1185 to 1600, and is normally divided into two sections, roughly equivalent to the Kamakura period and the Muromachi period , respectively. The later forms of Late Middle Japanese are the first to be described by non-native sources, in this case the Jesuit and Franciscan missionaries; and thus there

9360-433: The fricative [h] as an allophone of /k/ . Authors including Chomsky and Halle group [ f ] and [ v ] as sibilants. However, they do not have the grooved articulation and high frequencies of other sibilants, and most phoneticians continue to group them together with bilabial [ ɸ ] , [ β ] and (inter)dental [ θ ] , [ ð ] as non-sibilant anterior fricatives. For

9480-432: The fricatives /f, v, h/ . Also, almost all Eastern Polynesian languages have no sibilants but do have the fricatives /v/ and/or /f/ : Māori , Hawaiian , Tahitian , Rapa Nui , most Cook Islands Māori dialects, Marquesan , and Tuamotuan . Tamil only has the sibilant /ʂ/ and fricative /f/ in loanwords, and they are frequently replaced by native sounds. The sibilants [s, ɕ] exist as allophones of /t͡ɕ/ and

9600-609: The genitive particle ga remains in intentionally archaic speech. Early Middle Japanese is the Japanese of the Heian period , from 794 to 1185. It formed the basis for the literary standard of Classical Japanese , which remained in common use until the early 20th century. During this time, Japanese underwent numerous phonological developments, in many cases instigated by an influx of Chinese loanwords . These included phonemic length distinction for both consonants and vowels , palatal consonants (e.g. kya ) and labial consonant clusters (e.g. kwa ), and closed syllables . This had

9720-486: The grooved vs. hushing tongue shape so as to maximize the differences. However, the palato-alveolar sibilants in the Northwest Caucasian languages such as Ubykh are an exception. These sounds have the tongue tip resting directly against the lower teeth, which gives the sounds a quality that Catford describes as "hissing-hushing". Ladefoged and Maddieson term this a " closed laminal postalveolar" articulation, and transcribe them (following Catford) as [ŝ, ẑ] , although this

9840-453: The languages of the original Jōmon inhabitants, including the ancestor of the modern Ainu language . Because writing had yet to be introduced from China, there is no direct evidence, and anything that can be discerned about this period must be based on internal reconstruction from Old Japanese , or comparison with the Ryukyuan languages and Japanese dialects . The Chinese writing system

9960-449: The languages. Okinawan Japanese is a variant of Standard Japanese influenced by the Ryūkyūan languages, and is the primary dialect spoken among young people in the Ryukyu Islands . Modern Japanese has become prevalent nationwide (including the Ryūkyū islands) due to education , mass media , and an increase in mobility within Japan, as well as economic integration. Japanese is a member of

10080-427: The large quantity of English loanwords, modern Japanese has developed a distinction between [tɕi] and [ti] , and [dʑi] and [di] , with the latter in each pair only found in loanwords. Although Japanese is spoken almost exclusively in Japan, it has also been spoken outside of the country. Before and during World War II , through Japanese annexation of Taiwan and Korea , as well as partial occupation of China ,

10200-436: The latter probably through Amerindian influence, and alveolar and dorsal i.e. [ɕ ʑ cɕ ɟʑ] proper in Japanese ). Only a few languages with sibilants lack the hissing type. Middle Vietnamese is normally reconstructed with two sibilant fricatives, both hushing (one retroflex, one alveolo-palatal). Some languages have only a single hushing sibilant and no hissing sibilant. That occurs in southern Peninsular Spanish dialects of

10320-698: The lips are compressed throughout, and the sibilant may be followed by normal labialization upon release. (That is, there is a contrast among s, sw, ȿ, ȿw .) In Tsonga, the whistling effect is weak; the lips are narrowed but also the tongue is retroflex . Tswa may be similar. In Changana, the lips are rounded (protruded), but so is /s/ in the sequence /usu/, so there is evidently some distinct phonetic phenomenon occurring here that has yet to be formally identified and described. Not including differences in manner of articulation or secondary articulation , some languages have as many as four different types of sibilants. For example, Northern Qiang and Southern Qiang have

10440-415: The lower teeth is a hollow area (or pit) in the lower surface of the mouth. When the tongue tip rests in this hollow area, there is an empty space below the tongue (a sublingual cavity ), which results in a relatively duller sound. When the tip of the tongue rests against the lower teeth, there is no sublingual cavity, resulting in a sharper sound. Usually, the position of the tip of the tongue correlates with

10560-434: The middle of "miss you". Sibilants can be made at any coronal articulation , i.e. the tongue can contact the upper side of the mouth anywhere from the upper teeth ( dental ) to the hard palate ( palatal ), with the in-between articulations being denti-alveolar , alveolar and postalveolar . The tongue can contact the upper side of the mouth with the very tip of the tongue (an apical articulation, e.g. [ʃ̺] ); with

10680-560: The notation s̠, ṣ is sometimes reversed; either may also be called 'retroflex' and written ʂ .) ^1 ⟨ ŝ ẑ ⟩ is an ad-hoc transcription. The old IPA letters ⟨ ʆ ʓ ⟩ are also available. ^2 These sounds are usually just transcribed ⟨ ʂ ʐ ⟩ . Apical postalveolar and subapical palatal sibilants do not contrast in any language, but if necessary, apical postalveolars can be transcribed with an apical diacritic, as ⟨ s̠̺ z̠̺ ⟩ or ⟨ ʂ̺ ʐ̺ ⟩ . Ladefoged resurrects

10800-598: The old retroflex sub-dot for apical retroflexes, ⟨ ṣ ẓ ⟩ Also seen in the literature on e.g. Hindi and Norwegian is ⟨ ᶘ ᶚ ⟩ – the domed articulation of [ʃ ʒ] precludes a subapical realization. Whistled sibilants occur phonemically in several southern Bantu languages, the best known being Shona . However, they also occur in speech pathology and may be caused by dental prostheses or orthodontics. The whistled sibilants of Shona have been variously described—as labialized but not velarized, as retroflex, etc., but none of these features are required for

10920-425: The only strict rule of word order is that the verb must be placed at the end of a sentence (possibly followed by sentence-end particles). This is because Japanese sentence elements are marked with particles that identify their grammatical functions. The basic sentence structure is topic–comment . For example, Kochira wa Tanaka-san desu ( こちらは田中さんです ). kochira ("this") is the topic of the sentence, indicated by

11040-525: The otherwise IPA transcription of Shona in Doke (1967), the whistled sibilants are transcribed with the non-IPA letters ⟨ ȿ ɀ ⟩ and ⟨ tȿ dɀ ⟩ . Besides Shona, whistled sibilants have been reported as phonemes in Kalanga , Tsonga , Changana , Tswa —all of which are Southern African languages—and Tabasaran . The articulation of whistled sibilants may differ between languages. In Shona,

11160-470: The out-group gives a benefit to the in-group, and "up" to indicate the in-group gives a benefit to the out-group. Here, the in-group includes the speaker and the out-group does not, and their boundary depends on context. For example, oshiete moratta ( 教えてもらった ) (literally, "explaining got" with a benefit from the out-group to the in-group) means "[he/she/they] explained [it] to [me/us]". Similarly, oshiete ageta ( 教えてあげた ) (literally, "explaining gave" with

11280-433: The palato-alveolar appearing in the laminal "closed" variation) but also both the palato-alveolars and alveolo-palatals could additionally appear labialized . Besides, there was a five-way manner distinction among voiceless and voiced fricatives, voiceless and voiced affricates, and ejective affricates. (The three labialized palato-alveolar affricates were missing, which is why the total was 27, not 30.) The Bzyp dialect of

11400-415: The particle wa . The verb desu is a copula , commonly translated as "to be" or "it is" (though there are other verbs that can be translated as "to be"), though technically it holds no meaning and is used to give a sentence 'politeness'. As a phrase, Tanaka-san desu is the comment. This sentence literally translates to "As for this person, (it) is Mx Tanaka." Thus Japanese, like many other Asian languages,

11520-477: The proposed larger Altaic family, or to various Southeast Asian languages , especially Austronesian . None of these proposals have gained wide acceptance (and the Altaic family itself is now considered controversial). As it stands, only the link to Ryukyuan has wide support. Other theories view the Japanese language as an early creole language formed through inputs from at least two distinct language groups, or as

11640-450: The related Abkhaz language also has a similar inventory. Some languages have four types when palatalization is considered. Polish is one example, with both palatalized and non-palatalized laminal denti-alveolars, laminal postalveolar (or "flat retroflex"), and alveolo-palatal ( [s̪ z̪] [s̪ʲ z̪ʲ] [s̠ z̠] [ɕ ʑ] ). Russian has the same surface contrasts, but the alveolo-palatals are arguably not phonemic. They occur only geminate, and

11760-453: The result that there are many sibilant types that contrast in various languages. Sibilants are louder than their non-sibilant counterparts, and most of their acoustic energy occurs at higher frequencies than non-sibilant fricatives—usually around 8,000 Hz. All sibilants are coronal consonants (made with the tip or front part of the tongue). However, there is a great deal of variety among sibilants as to tongue shape, point of contact on

11880-453: The retroflex consonants never occur geminate, which suggests that both are allophones of the same phoneme. Somewhat more common are languages with three sibilant types, including one hissing and two hushing. As with Polish and Russian, the two hushing types are usually postalveolar and alveolo-palatal since these are the two most distinct from each other. Mandarin Chinese is an example of such

12000-459: The same structure as affirmative sentences, but with intonation rising at the end. In the formal register, the question particle -ka is added. For example, ii desu ( いいです ) "It is OK" becomes ii desu-ka ( いいですか。 ) "Is it OK?". In a more informal tone sometimes the particle -no ( の ) is added instead to show a personal interest of the speaker: Dōshite konai-no? "Why aren't (you) coming?". Some simple queries are formed simply by mentioning

12120-498: The sound of a sibilant consonant, or obstacle fricatives or affricates , which refers to the critical role of the teeth in producing the sound as an obstacle to the airstream. Non-sibilant fricatives and affricates produce their characteristic sound directly with the tongue or lips etc. and the place of contact in the mouth, without secondary involvement of the teeth. The characteristic intensity of sibilants means that small variations in tongue shape and position are perceivable, with

12240-454: The sounds. Using the Extended IPA , Shona sv and zv may be transcribed ⟨ s͎ ⟩ and ⟨ z͎ ⟩ . Other transcriptions seen include purely labialized ⟨ s̫ ⟩ and ⟨ z̫ ⟩ (Ladefoged and Maddieson 1996) and labially co-articulated ⟨ sᶲ ⟩ and ⟨ zᵝ ⟩ (or ⟨ s͡ɸ ⟩ and ⟨ z͜β ⟩ ). In

12360-797: The state as at the time the constitution was written, many of the elders participating in the process had been educated in Japanese during the South Seas Mandate over the island shown by the 1958 census of the Trust Territory of the Pacific that found that 89% of Palauans born between 1914 and 1933 could speak and read Japanese, but as of the 2005 Palau census there were no residents of Angaur that spoke Japanese at home. Japanese dialects typically differ in terms of pitch accent , inflectional morphology , vocabulary , and particle usage. Some even differ in vowel and consonant inventories, although this

12480-476: The street. (grammatically incorrect insertion of a pronoun) But one can grammatically say essentially the same thing in Japanese: 驚いた彼は道を走っていった。 Transliteration: Odoroita kare wa michi o hashitte itta. (grammatically correct) This is partly because these words evolved from regular nouns, such as kimi "you" ( 君 "lord"), anata "you" ( あなた "that side, yonder"), and boku "I" ( 僕 "servant"). This

12600-603: The suffix -ly follows a root ending in -l or -ll, as in: but not In some varieties of Welsh English , the process takes place indiscriminately between vowels, e.g. in money [ˈmɜn.niː] but it also applies with graphemic duplication (thus, orthographically dictated), e.g. butter [ˈbɜt̚.tə] In French, gemination is usually not phonologically relevant and therefore does not allow words to be distinguished: it mostly corresponds to an accent of insistence ( c'est terrifiant realised [ˈtɛʁ.ʁi.fjɑ̃] ), or meets hyper-correction criteria: one "corrects" one's pronunciation, despite

12720-416: The surface just behind the tip, called the blade of the tongue (a laminal articulation, e.g. [ʃ̻] ); or with the underside of the tip (a subapical articulation). Apical and subapical articulations are always tongue-up , with the tip of the tongue above the teeth, while laminal articulations can be either tongue-up or tongue-down , with the tip of the tongue behind the lower teeth. This distinction

12840-473: The tongue, and point of contact on the upper side of the mouth. The following variables affect sibilant sound quality, and, along with their possible values, are ordered from sharpest (highest-pitched) to dullest (lowest-pitched): Generally, the values of the different variables co-occur so as to produce an overall sharper or duller sound. For example, a laminal denti-alveolar grooved sibilant occurs in Polish , and

12960-669: The topic with an interrogative intonation to call for the hearer's attention: Kore wa? "(What about) this?"; O-namae wa? ( お名前は? ) "(What's your) name?". Negatives are formed by inflecting the verb. For example, Pan o taberu ( パンを食べる。 ) "I will eat bread" or "I eat bread" becomes Pan o tabenai ( パンを食べない。 ) "I will not eat bread" or "I do not eat bread". Plain negative forms are i -adjectives (see below) and inflect as such, e.g. Pan o tabenakatta ( パンを食べなかった。 ) "I did not eat bread". Sibilant Sibilants (from Latin : sībilāns  : 'hissing') are fricative consonants of higher amplitude and pitch , made by directing

13080-419: The two consonants are the moraic nasal followed by a homorganic consonant. Japanese also includes a pitch accent , which is not represented in moraic writing; for example [haꜜ.ɕi] ("chopsticks") and [ha.ɕiꜜ] ("bridge") are both spelled はし ( hashi ) , and are only differentiated by the tone contour. Japanese word order is classified as subject–object–verb . Unlike many Indo-European languages ,

13200-577: The two methods were both used in writing until the 1940s. Bungo still has some relevance for historians, literary scholars, and lawyers (many Japanese laws that survived World War II are still written in bungo , although there are ongoing efforts to modernize their language). Kōgo is the dominant method of both speaking and writing Japanese today, although bungo grammar and vocabulary are occasionally used in modern Japanese for effect. The 1982 state constitution of Angaur , Palau , names Japanese along with Palauan and English as an official language of

13320-472: The two terms (''hyōjungo'' and ''kyōtsūgo'') are almost the same. Hyōjungo or kyōtsūgo is a conception that forms the counterpart of dialect. This normative language was born after the Meiji Restoration ( 明治維新 , meiji ishin , 1868) from the language spoken in the higher-class areas of Tokyo (see Yamanote ). Hyōjungo is taught in schools and used on television and in official communications. It

13440-510: The uses of diacritics. In Gurmukhi the diacritic is called the [[[ੱ|áddak]]] Error: {{Transliteration}}: transliteration text not Latin script ( help ) which is written before the geminated consonant and is mandatory. In contrast, the shadda , which is used to represent gemination in the Shahmukhi script , is not necessarily written, retaining the tradition of the original Arabic script and Persian language , where diacritics are usually omitted from writing, except to clear ambiguity, and

13560-453: The usual phonology, to be closer to a realization that one imagines to be more correct: thus, the word illusion is sometimes pronounced [il.lyˈzjɔ̃] by influence of the spelling. However, gemination is distinctive in a few cases. Statements such as elle a dit ('she said') ~ elle l'a dit ('she said it') /ɛl a di/ ~ /ɛl l‿a di/ can commonly be distinguished by gemination. In a more sustained pronunciation, gemination distinguishes

13680-407: The verb (e.g. yonde for earlier yomite ), the -k- in the final mora of adjectives drops out ( shiroi for earlier shiroki ); and some forms exist where modern standard Japanese has retained the earlier form (e.g. hayaku > hayau > hayɔɔ , where modern Japanese just has hayaku , though the alternative form is preserved in the standard greeting o-hayō gozaimasu "good morning"; this ending

13800-621: The world have this pattern. Perhaps most common is the pattern, as in English and Arabic, with alveolar and palato-alveolar sibilants. Modern northern peninsular Spanish has a single apico-alveolar sibilant fricative [s̠] , as well as a single palato-alveolar sibilant affricate [tʃ] . However, there are also languages with alveolar and apical retroflex sibilants (such as Standard Vietnamese ) and with alveolar and alveolo-palatal postalveolars (e.g. alveolar and laminal palatalized [ʃ ʒ tʃ dʒ] i.e. [ʃʲ ʒʲ tʃʲ dʒʲ] in Catalan and Brazilian Portuguese ,

13920-548: The world. Since Japanese first gained the consideration of linguists in the late 19th century, attempts have been made to show its genealogical relation to languages or language families such as Ainu , Korean , Chinese , Tibeto-Burman , Uralic , Altaic (or Ural-Altaic ), Austroasiatic , Austronesian and Dravidian . At the fringe, some linguists have even suggested a link to Indo-European languages , including Greek , or to Sumerian . Main modern theories try to link Japanese either to northern Asian languages, like Korean or

14040-535: Was based on 12- to 20-second-long recordings of 135 to 244 phonemes , which 42 students listened to and translated word-for-word. The listeners were all Keio University students who grew up in the Kanto region . There are some language islands in mountain villages or isolated islands such as Hachijō-jima island , whose dialects are descended from Eastern Old Japanese . Dialects of the Kansai region are spoken or known by many Japanese, and Osaka dialect in particular

14160-1012: Was distinctive, as in a n us 'old woman' vs. a nn us 'year'. Vowel length was also distinctive in Latin until about the fourth century, and was reflected in the orthography with an apex . Geminates inherited from Latin still exist in Italian , in which [ˈanno] anno and [ˈaːno] ano contrast with regard to /nn/ and /n/ as in Latin. It has been almost completely lost in French and completely in Romanian . In West Iberian languages , former Latin geminate consonants often evolved to new phonemes, including some instances of nasal vowels in Portuguese and Old Galician as well as most cases of /ɲ/ and /ʎ/ in Spanish, but phonetic length of both consonants and vowels

14280-668: Was imported to Japan from Baekje around the start of the fifth century, alongside Buddhism. The earliest texts were written in Classical Chinese , although some of these were likely intended to be read as Japanese using the kanbun method, and show influences of Japanese grammar such as Japanese word order. The earliest text, the Kojiki , dates to the early eighth century, and was written entirely in Chinese characters, which are used to represent, at different times, Chinese, kanbun , and Old Japanese. As in other texts from this period,

14400-465: Was lost immediately following its composition.) This set of morae shrank to 67 in Early Middle Japanese , though some were added through Chinese influence. Man'yōgana also has a symbol for /je/ , which merges with /e/ before the end of the period. Several fossilizations of Old Japanese grammatical elements remain in the modern language – the genitive particle tsu (superseded by modern no )

#531468