Misplaced Pages

Consonant cluster

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.

In linguistics , a consonant cluster , consonant sequence or consonant compound , is a group of consonants which have no intervening vowel . In English, for example, the groups /spl/ and /ts/ are consonant clusters in the word splits . In the education field it is variously called a consonant cluster or a consonant blend .

#323676

101-823: Some linguists argue that the term can be properly applied only to those consonant clusters that occur within one syllable . Others claim that the concept is more useful when it includes consonant sequences across syllable boundaries. According to the former definition, the longest consonant clusters in the word extra would be /ks/ and /tr/ , whereas the latter allows /kstr/ , which is phonetically [kst̠ɹ̠̊˔ʷ] in some accents. Each language has an associated set of phonotactic constraints . Languages' phonotactics differ as to what consonant clusters they permit. Many languages are more restrictive than English in terms of consonant clusters, and some forbid consonant clusters entirely. For example, Hawaiian , like most Malayo-Polynesian languages, forbid consonant clusters entirely. Japanese

202-572: A long vowel sound. This is the case in Finnish and Estonian , for instance, where ⟨uu⟩ represents a longer version of the vowel denoted by ⟨u⟩ , ⟨ää⟩ represents a longer version of the vowel denoted by ⟨ä⟩ , and so on. In Middle English , the sequences ⟨ee⟩ and ⟨oo⟩ were used in a similar way, to represent lengthened "e" and "o" sounds respectively; both spellings have been retained in modern English orthography , but

303-448: A cluster of two consonants: /ŋθ/ (although it may be pronounced /ŋkθ/ instead, as ⟨ng⟩ followed by a voiceless consonant in the same syllable often does); lights with a silent digraph ⟨gh⟩ followed by a cluster ⟨t⟩ , ⟨s⟩ : /ts/ ; and compound words such as sightscreen /ˈsaɪtskriːn/ or catchphrase /ˈkætʃfreɪz/ . Not all consonant clusters are distributed equally among

404-409: A digraph or a combination of letters. They are the most common combinations, but extreme regional differences exists, especially those of the eastern dialects . A noteworthy difference is the aspiration of ⟨rs⟩ in eastern dialects, where it corresponds to ⟨skj⟩ and ⟨sj⟩ . Among many young people, especially in the western regions of Norway and in or around

505-540: A final [j] sound can be moved to the next syllable in enchainement, sometimes with a gemination: e.g., non ne ho mai avuti ('I've never had any of them') is broken into syllables as [non.neˈɔ.ma.jaˈvuːti] and io ci vado e lei anche ('I go there and she does as well') is realized as [jo.tʃiˈvaːdo.e.lɛjˈjaŋ.ke] . A related phenomenon, called consonant mutation, is found in the Celtic languages like Irish and Welsh, whereby unwritten (but historical) final consonants affect

606-517: A glottal stop be inserted between a word and a following, putatively vowel-initial word. Yet such words are perceived to begin with a vowel in German but a glottal stop in Arabic. The reason for this has to do with other properties of the two languages. For example, a glottal stop does not occur in other situations in German, e.g. before a consonant or at the end of word. On the other hand, in Arabic, not only does

707-499: A glottal stop is inserted – indicates whether the word should be considered to have a null onset. For example, many Romance languages such as Spanish never insert such a glottal stop, while English does so only some of the time, depending on factors such as conversation speed; in both cases, this suggests that the words in question are truly vowel-initial. But there are exceptions here, too. For example, standard German (excluding many southern accents) and Arabic both require that

808-435: A glottal stop occur in such situations (e.g. Classical /saʔala/ "he asked", /raʔj/ "opinion", /dˤawʔ/ "light"), but it occurs in alternations that are clearly indicative of its phonemic status (cf. Classical /kaːtib/ "writer" vs. /mak tuːb/ "written", /ʔaːkil/ "eater" vs. /maʔkuːl/ "eaten"). In other words, while the glottal stop is predictable in German (inserted only if a stressed syllable would otherwise begin with

909-439: A linear one, between the syllable constituents. One hierarchical model groups the syllable nucleus and coda into an intermediate level, the rime . The hierarchical model accounts for the role that the nucleus + coda constituent plays in verse (i.e., rhyming words such as cat and bat are formed by matching both the nucleus and coda, or the entire rime), and for the distinction between heavy and light syllables , which plays

1010-455: A medial contrast between /i/ and /j/ , where the /i/ functions phonologically as a glide rather than as part of the nucleus. In addition, many reconstructions of both Old and Middle Chinese include complex medials such as /rj/ , /ji/ , /jw/ and /jwi/ . The medial groups phonologically with the rime rather than the onset, and the combination of medial and rime is collectively known as the final . Some linguists, especially when discussing

1111-621: A phoneme are not always adjacent. This is the case with English silent e . For example, the sequence a_e has the sound /eɪ/ in English cake. This is the result of three historical sound changes: cake was originally /kakə/ , the open syllable /ka/ came to be pronounced with a long vowel , and later the final schwa dropped off, leaving /kaːk/ . Later still, the vowel /aː/ became /eɪ/ . There are six such digraphs in English, ⟨a_e, e_e, i_e, o_e, u_e, y_e⟩ . However, alphabets may also be designed with discontinuous digraphs. In

SECTION 10

#1732883607324

1212-456: A phonemic glottal stop (the sound in the middle of English uh-oh or, in some dialects, the double T in button , represented in the IPA as /ʔ/ ). In English, a word that begins with a vowel may be pronounced with an epenthetic glottal stop when following a pause, though the glottal stop may not be a phoneme in the language. Few languages make a phonemic distinction between a word beginning with

1313-414: A post-coda sibilant, "s". Clusters of -ps, -ts, -ks, were then formed at the end of syllables. These clusters eventually collapsed into "-ts" or "-s", before disappearing altogether, leaving elements of diphthongisation in more modern varieties. Old Vietnamese also had a rich inventory of initial clusters, but these were slowly merged with plain initials during Middle Vietnamese, and some have developed into

1414-542: A role in phonological processes such as, for example, sound change in Old English scipu and wordu , where in a process called high vowel deletion (HVD), the nominative/accusative plural of single light-syllable roots (like "*scip-") got a "u" ending in OE, whereas heavy syllable roots (like "*word-") would not, giving "scip-u" but "word-∅". In some traditional descriptions of certain languages such as Cree and Ojibwe ,

1515-648: A romanisation of Russian ⟨ ж ⟩ . The capitalisation of digraphs can vary, e.g. ⟨sz⟩ in Polish is capitalized ⟨Sz⟩ and ⟨kj⟩ in Norwegian is capitalized ⟨Kj⟩ , while ⟨ ij ⟩ in Dutch is capitalized ⟨IJ⟩ and word initial ⟨dt⟩ in Irish is capitalized ⟨dT⟩ . Digraphs may develop into ligatures , but this

1616-602: A row per syllable. Finnish has initial consonant clusters natively only on South-Western dialects and on foreign loans, and only clusters of three inside the word are allowed. Most spoken languages and dialects, however, are more permissive. In Burmese , consonant clusters of only up to three consonants (the initial and two medials—two written forms of /-j-/ , /-w-/ ) at the initial onset are allowed in writing and only two (the initial and one medial) are pronounced; these clusters are restricted to certain letters. Some Burmese dialects allow for clusters of up to four consonants (with

1717-467: A significant number forbid any heavy syllable. Some languages strive for constant syllable weight; for example, in stressed, non-final syllables in Italian , short vowels co-occur with closed syllables while long vowels co-occur with open syllables, so that all such syllables are heavy (not light or superheavy). The difference between heavy and light frequently determines which syllables receive stress – this

1818-451: A single character in the writing system of a language, like ⟨ ch ⟩ in Spanish chico and ocho . Other digraphs represent phonemes that can also be represented by single characters. A digraph that shares its pronunciation with a single character may be a relic from an earlier period of the language when the digraph had a different pronunciation, or may represent a distinction that

1919-444: A single syllable (like English dog ) is called a monosyllable (and is said to be monosyllabic ). Similar terms include disyllable (and disyllabic ; also bisyllable and bisyllabic ) for a word of two syllables; trisyllable (and trisyllabic ) for a word of three syllables; and polysyllable (and polysyllabic ), which may refer either to a word of more than three syllables or to any word of more than one syllable. Syllable

2020-489: A specific place in the alphabet , separate from that of the sequence of characters that composes them, for purposes of orthography and collation : Most other languages, including most of the Romance languages, treat digraphs as combinations of separate letters for alphabetization purposes. English has both homogeneous digraphs (doubled letters) and heterogeneous digraphs (digraphs consisting of two different letters). Those of

2121-493: A syllabic nucleus. A few languages have so-called syllabic fricatives , also known as fricative vowels , at the phonemic level. (In the context of Chinese phonology , the related but non-synonymous term apical vowel is commonly used.) Mandarin Chinese is famous for having such sounds in at least some of its dialects, for example the pinyin syllables sī shī rī , usually pronounced [sź̩ ʂʐ̩́ ʐʐ̩́] , respectively. Though, like

SECTION 20

#1732883607324

2222-495: A syllable break, for example in the word "understood" ⟨ /ʌndərˈstʊd/ ⟩ (though the syllable boundary may still be explicitly marked with a full stop, e.g. ⟨ /ʌn.dər.ˈstʊd/ ⟩). When a word space comes in the middle of a syllable (that is, when a syllable spans words), a tie bar ⟨ ‿ ⟩ can be used for liaison , as in the French combination les amis ⟨ /lɛ.z‿a.mi/ ⟩. The liaison tie

2323-440: A syllable-final /r/ , which is not normally found, while /hʌ.ri/ gives a syllable-final short stressed vowel, which is also non-occurring. Arguments can be made in favour of one solution or the other: A general rule has been proposed that states that "Subject to certain conditions ..., consonants are syllabified with the more strongly stressed of two flanking syllables", while many other phonologists prefer to divide syllables with

2424-452: A third type of superheavy syllable , which consists of VVC syllables (with both a branching nucleus and rime) or VCC syllables (with a coda consisting of two or more consonants) or both. In moraic theory , heavy syllables are said to have two moras, while light syllables are said to have one and superheavy syllables are said to have three. Japanese phonology is generally described this way. Many languages forbid superheavy syllables, while

2525-477: A true geminate consonant in modern English; this may occur when two instances of the same consonant come from different morphemes , for example ⟨nn⟩ in unnatural ( un + natural ) or ⟨tt⟩ in cattail ( cat + tail ). In some cases, the sound represented by a doubled consonant letter is distinguished in some other way than length from the sound of the corresponding single consonant letter: In several European writing systems, including

2626-621: A vowel and a word beginning with a glottal stop followed by a vowel, since the distinction will generally only be audible following another word. However, Maltese and some Polynesian languages do make such a distinction, as in Hawaiian /ahi/ ('fire') and /ʔahi / ← /kahi/ ('tuna') and Maltese /∅/ ← Arabic /h/ and Maltese /k~ʔ/ ← Arabic /q/ . Ashkenazi and Sephardi Hebrew may commonly ignore א , ה and ע , and Arabic forbid empty onsets. The names Israel , Abel , Abraham , Omar , Abdullah , and Iraq appear not to have onsets in

2727-538: A vowel), the same sound is a regular consonantal phoneme in Arabic. The status of this consonant in the respective writing systems corresponds to this difference: there is no reflex of the glottal stop in German orthography , but there is a letter in the Arabic alphabet ( Hamza ( ء )). The writing system of a language may not correspond with the phonological analysis of the language in terms of its handling of (potentially) null onsets. For example, in some languages written in

2828-474: A whole number of syllables: for example, the word ignite is made of two syllables: ig and nite . Syllabic writing began several hundred years before the first letters . The earliest recorded syllables are on tablets written around 2800 BC in the Sumerian city of Ur . This shift from pictograms to syllables has been called "the most important advance in the history of writing ". A word that consists of

2929-402: A word-final consonant to a vowel beginning the word immediately following it forms a regular part of the phonetics of some languages, including Spanish, Hungarian, and Turkish. Thus, in Spanish, the phrase los hombres ('the men') is pronounced [loˈsom.bɾes] , Hungarian az ember ('the human') as [ɒˈzɛm.bɛr] , and Turkish nefret ettim ('I hated it') as [nefˈɾe.tet.tim] . In Italian,

3030-417: Is a syllabic consonant . In most Germanic languages , lax vowels can occur only in closed syllables. Therefore, these vowels are also called checked vowels , as opposed to the tense vowels that are called free vowels because they can occur even in open syllables. The notion of syllable is challenged by languages that allow long strings of obstruents without any intervening vowel or sonorant . By far

3131-449: Is a basic unit of organization within a sequence of speech sounds , such as within a word, typically made up of a syllable nucleus (most often a vowel ) with optional initial and final margins (typically, consonants ). Syllables are often considered the phonological "building blocks" of words . They can influence the rhythm of a language, its prosody , its poetic metre and its stress patterns. Speech can usually be divided up into

Consonant cluster - Misplaced Pages Continue

3232-663: Is a digraph ⟨zh⟩ that represents [z] in most dialects, but [h] in Vannetais. Similarly, the Saintongeais dialect of French has a digraph ⟨jh⟩ that represents [h] in words that correspond to [ʒ] in standard French. Similarly, Catalan has a digraph ⟨ix⟩ that represents [ʃ] in Eastern Catalan , but [jʃ] or [js] in Western Catalan – Valencian . The pair of letters making up

3333-536: Is a distinct concept: a ligature involves the graphical fusion of two characters into one, e.g. when ⟨o⟩ and ⟨e⟩ become ⟨œ⟩ , e.g. as in French cœur "heart". Digraphs may consist of two different characters (heterogeneous digraphs) or two instances of the same character (homogeneous digraphs). In the latter case, they are generally called double (or doubled ) letters . Doubled vowel letters are commonly used to indicate

3434-528: Is a letter that represents a plosive most accurately pronounced by trying to say /g/ and /b/ at the same time. Modern Slavic languages written in the Cyrillic alphabet make little use of digraphs apart from ⟨дж⟩ for /dʐ/ , ⟨дз⟩ for /dz/ (in Ukrainian, Belarusian, and Bulgarian), and ⟨жж⟩ and ⟨зж⟩ for the uncommon Russian phoneme /ʑː/ . In Russian,

3535-417: Is a root meaning 'to shine, to be bright' and is also present in ⟨glee⟩ , ⟨gleam⟩ , and ⟨glade⟩ . Consonant clusters can also originate from assimilation of a consonant with a vowel. In many Slavic languages, the combination mji, mje, mja etc. regularly gave mlji, mlje, mlja etc. Compare Russian zemlyá , which had this change, with Polish ziemia , which lacks

3636-947: Is almost as strict, but allows a sequence of a nasal consonant plus another consonant, as in Honshū [hoꜜɰ̃ɕɯː] (the name of the largest island of Japan). (Palatalized consonants, such as [kʲ] in Tōkyō [toːkʲoː] , are single consonants.) It also permits a syllable to end in a consonant as long as the next syllable begins with the same consonant. Standard Arabic forbids initial consonant clusters and more than two consecutive consonants in other positions, as do most other Semitic languages , although Modern Israeli Hebrew permits initial two-consonant clusters (e.g. pkak "cap"; dlaat "pumpkin"), and Moroccan Arabic , under Berber influence, allows strings of several consonants. Like most Mon–Khmer languages , Khmer permits only initial consonant clusters with up to three consonants in

3737-469: Is also rare, but occurs in Russian words such as мха ( /mxa/ ). Consonant clusters at the ends of syllables are less common but follow the same principles. Clusters are more likely to begin with a liquid, approximant, or nasal and end with a fricative, affricate, or stop, such as in English "world" /wə(ɹ)ld/ . Yet again, there are exceptions, such as English "lapse" /læps/ . Syllable A syllable

3838-427: Is also used to join lexical words into phonological words , for example hot dog ⟨ /ˈhɒt‿dɒɡ/ ⟩. A Greek sigma, ⟨σ⟩ , is used as a wild card for 'syllable', and a dollar/peso sign, ⟨$ ⟩ , marks a syllable boundary where the usual fullstop might be misunderstood. For example, ⟨σσ⟩ is a pair of syllables, and ⟨V$ ⟩ is a syllable-final vowel. In

3939-424: Is an Anglo-Norman variation of Old French sillabe , from Latin syllaba , from Koine Greek συλλαβή syllabḗ ( Greek pronunciation: [sylːabɛ̌ː] ). συλλαβή means "the taken together", referring to letters that are taken together to make a single sound. συλλαβή is a verbal noun from the verb συλλαμβάνω syllambánō , a compound of the preposition σύν sýn "with" and

4040-496: Is analysed as CCCCCCCCVC. Many Slavic languages may manifest almost as formidable numbers of consecutive consonants, such as in the Czech tongue twister Strč prst skrz krk ( pronounced [str̩tʃ pr̩st skr̩s kr̩k] ), meaning 'stick a finger through the neck', the Slovak words štvrť /ʃtvr̩c/ ("quarter"), and žblnknutie /ʒbl̩ŋknucɪɛ̯/ ("clunk"; "flop"), and

4141-512: Is either a closed syllable that ends in a consonant, or a syllable with a branching nucleus , i.e. a long vowel or diphthong . The name is a metaphor, based on the nucleus or coda having lines that branch in a tree diagram. In some languages, heavy syllables include both VV (branching nucleus) and VC (branching rime) syllables, contrasted with V, which is a light syllable . In other languages, only VV syllables are considered heavy, while both VC and V syllables are light. Some languages distinguish

Consonant cluster - Misplaced Pages Continue

4242-544: Is made only in certain dialects , like the English ⟨ wh ⟩ . Some such digraphs are used for purely etymological reasons, like ⟨ ph ⟩ in French. In some orthographies, digraphs (and occasionally trigraphs ) are considered individual letters , which means that they have their own place in the alphabet and cannot be separated into their constituent places graphemes when sorting , abbreviating , or hyphenating words. Digraphs are used in some romanization schemes, e.g. ⟨ zh ⟩ as

4343-488: Is not, and sk- is possible but ks- is not. In Greek , however, both ks- and tl- are possible onsets, while contrarily in Classical Arabic no multiconsonant onsets are allowed at all. Some languages forbid null onsets . In these languages, words beginning in a vowel, like the English word at , are impossible. This is less strange than it may appear at first, as most such languages allow syllables to begin with

4444-407: Is pronounced without the /k/ . Final clusters of four consonants, as in angsts in other dialects ( /ˈæŋsts/ ), twelfths /ˈtwɛlfθs/ , sixths /ˈsɪksθs/ , bursts /ˈbɜːrsts/ (in rhotic accents ) and glimpsed /ˈɡlɪmpst/ , are more common. Within compound words, clusters of five consonants or more are possible (if cross-syllabic clusters are accepted), as in handspring /ˈhændsprɪŋ/ and in

4545-478: Is the case in Latin and Arabic , for example. The system of poetic meter in many classical languages, such as Classical Greek , Classical Latin , Old Tamil and Sanskrit , is based on syllable weight rather than stress (so-called quantitative rhythm or quantitative meter ). Syllabification is the separation of a word into syllables, whether spoken or written. In most languages, the actually spoken syllables are

4646-408: Is thus a matter of definition. Some letter pairs should not be interpreted as digraphs but appear because of compounding : hogshead and cooperate . They are often not marked in any way and so must be memorized as exceptions. Some authors, however, indicate it either by breaking up the digraph with a hyphen , as in hogs-head , co-operate , or with a trema mark , as in coöperate , but the use of

4747-421: Is used. One analysis would consider all vowel and consonant segments as syllable nuclei, another would consider only a small subset ( fricatives or sibilants ) as nuclei candidates, and another would simply deny the existence of syllables completely. However, when working with recordings rather than transcriptions, the syllables can be obvious in such languages, and native speakers have strong intuitions as to what

4848-419: Is usually the portion of a syllable from the first vowel to the end. For example, /æt/ is the rime of all of the words at , sat , and flat . However, the nucleus does not necessarily need to be a vowel in some languages, such as English. For instance, the rime of the second syllables of the words bottle and fiddle is just /l/ , a liquid consonant . Just as the rime branches into the nucleus and coda,

4949-422: The ⟨lj⟩ and ⟨nj⟩ are digraphs representing single consonants: [ʎ] and [ɲ] , respectively. In Dutch , clusters of six or even seven consonants are possible (e.g. angstschreeuw ("a scream of fear"), slechtstschrijvend ("writing the worst") and zachtstschrijdend ("treading the most softly")). Some Salishan languages exhibit long words with no vowels at all, such as

5050-591: The Armenian language , the digraph ու ⟨ou⟩ transcribes / u / , a convention that comes from Greek. The Georgian alphabet uses a few digraphs to write other languages. For example, in Svan , /ø/ is written ჳე ⟨we⟩ , and /y/ as ჳი ⟨wi⟩ . Modern Greek has the following digraphs: They are called "diphthongs" in Greek ; in classical times, most of them represented diphthongs , and

5151-515: The Arrernte language of central Australia may prohibit onsets altogether; if so, all syllables have the underlying shape VC(C). The difference between a syllable with a null onset and one beginning with a glottal stop is often purely a difference of phonological analysis, rather than the actual pronunciation of the syllable. In some cases, the pronunciation of a (putatively) vowel-initial word when following another word – particularly, whether or not

SECTION 50

#1732883607324

5252-474: The Great Vowel Shift and other historical sound changes mean that the modern pronunciations are quite different from the original ones. Doubled consonant letters can also be used to indicate a long or geminated consonant sound. In Italian , for example, consonants written double are pronounced longer than single ones. This was the original use of doubled consonant letters in Old English , but during

5353-537: The Latin alphabet , an initial glottal stop is left unwritten (see the German example); on the other hand, some languages written using non-Latin alphabets such as abjads and abugidas have a special zero consonant to represent a null onset. As an example, in Hangul , the alphabet of the Korean language , a null onset is represented with ㅇ at the left or top section of a grapheme , as in 역 "station", pronounced yeok , where

5454-461: The Middle English and Early Modern English period, phonemic consonant length was lost and a spelling convention developed in which a doubled consonant serves to indicate that a preceding vowel is to be pronounced short. In modern English, for example, the ⟨pp⟩ of tapping differentiates the first vowel sound from that of taping . In rare cases, doubled consonant letters represent

5555-543: The Nuxálk word /xɬpʼχʷɬtʰɬpʰɬːskʷʰt͡sʼ/ : he had had in his possession a bunchberry plant . It is extremely difficult to accurately classify which of these consonants may be acting as the syllable nucleus, and these languages challenge classical notions of exactly what constitutes a syllable . The same problem is encountered in the Northern Berber languages . There has been a trend to reduce and simplify consonant clusters in

5656-613: The Slovene word skrbstvo /skrbstʋo/ ("welfare"). However, the liquid consonants /r/ and /l/ can form syllable nuclei in West and South Slavic languages and behave phonologically as vowels in this case. An example of a true initial cluster is the Polish word wszczniesz ( /fʂt͡ʂɲɛʂ/ ("you will initiate"). In the Serbo-Croatian word opskrbljivanje /ɔpskr̩bʎiʋaɲɛ/ ("victualling")

5757-525: The Tatar Cyrillic alphabet , for example, the letter ю is used to write both /ju/ and /jy/ . Usually the difference is evident from the rest of the word, but when it is not, the sequence ю...ь is used for /jy/ , as in юнь /jyn/ 'cheap'. The Indic alphabets are distinctive for their discontinuous vowels, such as Thai เ...อ /ɤː/ in เกอ /kɤː/ . Technically, however, they may be considered diacritics , not full letters; whether they are digraphs

5858-428: The diphthong yeo is the nucleus and k is the coda. [REDACTED] The nucleus is usually the vowel in the middle of a syllable. Generally, every syllable requires a nucleus (sometimes called the peak ), and the minimal syllable consists only of a nucleus, as in the English words "eye" or "owe". The syllable nucleus is usually a vowel, in the form of a monophthong , diphthong , or triphthong , but sometimes

5959-575: The English one, the doubling of the letter ⟨c⟩ or ⟨k⟩ is represented as the heterogeneous digraph ⟨ck⟩ instead of ⟨cc⟩ or ⟨kk⟩ respectively. In native German words, the doubling of ⟨z⟩ , which corresponds to /ts/ , is replaced by the digraph ⟨tz⟩ . Some languages have a unified orthography with digraphs that represent distinct pronunciations in different dialects ( diaphonemes ). For example, in Breton there

6060-1068: The Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area , such as Chinese and Vietnamese . Old Chinese was known to contain additional medials such as /r/ and/or /l/ , which yielded retroflexion in Middle Chinese and today's Mandarin Chinese . The word 江 , read /tɕiɑŋ˥/ in Mandarin and /kɔːŋ˥⁻˥˧/ in Cantonese , is reconstructed as *klong or *krung in Old Chinese by Sinologists like Zhengzhang Shangfang , William H. Baxter , and Laurent Sagart . Additionally, initial clusters such as "tk" and "sn" were analysed in recent reconstructions of Old Chinese, and some were developed as palatalised sibilants . Similarly, in Thai , words with initial consonant clusters are commonly reduced in colloquial speech to pronounce only

6161-468: The SSP such as Proto-Indo-European /st/ and /spl/ (which many of its descendants have, including English). Certain consonants are more or less likely to appear in consonant clusters, especially in certain positions. The Tsou language of Taiwan has initial clusters such as /tf/ , which doesn't violate the SSP, but nonetheless is unusual in having the labio-dental /f/ in the second position. The cluster /mx/

SECTION 60

#1732883607324

6262-493: The Yorkshire place-name of Hampsthwaite /hæmpsθweɪt/ . It is important to distinguish clusters and digraphs . Clusters are made of two or more consonant sounds , while a digraph is a group of two consonant letters standing for a single sound. For example, in the word ship , the two letters of the digraph ⟨sh⟩ together represent the single consonant [ʃ] . Conversely, the letter ⟨x⟩ can produce

6363-408: The above definition. [REDACTED] In some theories of phonology, syllable structures are displayed as tree diagrams (similar to the trees found in some types of syntax). Not all phonologists agree that syllables have internal structure; in fact, some phonologists doubt the existence of the syllable as a theoretical entity. There are many arguments for a hierarchical relationship, rather than

6464-759: The addition of the /-l-/ medial, which can combine with the above-mentioned medials). At the other end of the scale, the Kartvelian languages of Georgia are drastically more permissive of consonant clustering. Clusters in Georgian of four, five or six consonants are not unusual—for instance, /brtʼqʼɛli/ ( flat ), /mt͡sʼvrtnɛli/ ( trainer ) and /prt͡skvna/ ( peeling )—and if grammatical affixes are used, it allows an eight-consonant cluster: /ɡvbrdɣvnis/ ( he's plucking us ), /gvprt͡skvni/ ( you peel us ). Consonants cannot appear as syllable nuclei in Georgian, so this syllable

6565-415: The apostrophe is seen in pinyin where 嫦娥 is written Chang'e because the g belongs to the final (-ang) of the first syllable, not to the initial of the second syllable. Without the apostrophe, Change would be understood as the syllable chan (final -an) followed by the syllable ge (initial g-). In some languages, certain digraphs and trigraphs are counted as distinct letters in themselves, and assigned to

6666-476: The basis of syllabification in writing too. Due to the very weak correspondence between sounds and letters in the spelling of modern English, for example, written syllabification in English has to be based mostly on etymological i.e. morphological instead of phonetic principles. English written syllables therefore do not correspond to the actually spoken syllables of the living language. Phonotactic rules determine which sounds are allowed or disallowed in each part of

6767-435: The beginning of a syllable, occurring before the nucleus . Most syllables have an onset. Syllables without an onset may be said to have an empty or zero onset – that is, nothing where the onset would be. Some languages restrict onsets to be only a single consonant, while others allow multiconsonant onsets according to various rules. For example, in English, onsets such as pr- , pl- and tr- are possible but tl-

6868-435: The change, both from Proto-Balto-Slavic *źemē. See Proto-Slavic language and History of Proto-Slavic for more information about this change. All languages differ in syllable structure and cluster template. A loanword from Adyghe in the extinct Ubykh language , psta ('to well up'), violates Ubykh's limit of two initial consonants. The English words sphere /ˈsfɪər/ and sphinx /ˈsfɪŋks/ , Greek loanwords, break

6969-471: The consonant clusters /ks/ (annex), /gz/ (exist), /kʃ/ (sexual), or /gʒ/ (some pronunciations of "luxury"). It is worth noting that ⟨x⟩ often produces sounds in two different syllables (following the general principle of saturating the subsequent syllable before assigning sounds to the preceding syllable). Also note a combination digraph and cluster as seen in length with two digraphs ⟨ng⟩ , ⟨th⟩ representing

7070-472: The consonant or consonants attached to the following syllable wherever possible. However, an alternative that has received some support is to treat an intervocalic consonant as ambisyllabic , i.e. belonging both to the preceding and to the following syllable: /hʌṛi/ . This is discussed in more detail in English phonology § Phonotactics . The onset (also known as anlaut ) is the consonant sound or sounds at

7171-454: The constituent sounds ( morae ) are usually indicated by digraphs, but some are indicated by a single letter, and some with a trigraph. The case of ambiguity is the syllabic ん , which is written as n (or sometimes m ), except before vowels or y where it is followed by an apostrophe as n’ . For example, the given name じゅんいちろう is romanized as Jun’ichirō, so that it is parsed as "Jun-i-chi-rou", rather than as "Ju-ni-chi-rou". A similar use of

7272-432: The diaeresis has declined in English within the last century. When it occurs in names such as Clapham , Townshend, and Hartshorne, it is never marked in any way. Positional alternative glyphs may help to disambiguate in certain cases: when round, ⟨s⟩ was used as a final variant of long ⟨ſ⟩ , and the English digraph for /ʃ/ would always be ⟨ſh⟩ . In romanization of Japanese ,

7373-403: The first syllable, but in the original Hebrew and Arabic forms they actually begin with various consonants: the semivowel / j / in יִשְׂרָאֵל yisra'él , the glottal fricative in / h / הֶבֶל heḇel , the glottal stop / ʔ / in אַבְרָהָם 'aḇrāhām , or the pharyngeal fricative / ʕ / in عُمَر ʿumar , عَبْدُ ٱللّٰ ʿabdu llāh , and عِرَاق ʿirāq . Conversely,

7474-454: The initial consonant of the following word. There can be disagreement about the location of some divisions between syllables in spoken language. The problems of dealing with such cases have been most commonly discussed with relation to English. In the case of a word such as hurry , the division may be /hʌr.i/ or /hʌ.ri/ , neither of which seems a satisfactory analysis for a non-rhotic accent such as RP (British English): /hʌr.i/ results in

7575-522: The initial consonant, such as the pronunciation of the word ครับ reducing from /kʰrap̚˦˥/ to /kʰap̚˦˥/ . Another element of consonant clusters in Old Chinese was analysed in coda and post-coda position. Some "departing tone" syllables have cognates in the "entering tone" syllables, which feature a -p, -t, -k in Middle Chinese and Southern Chinese varieties. The departing tone was analysed to feature

7676-602: The language. Other factors that affect clusters when loaned to other languages include speech rate, articulatory factors, and speech perceptivity. Bayley has added that social factors such as age, gender, and geographical locations of speakers can determine clusters when they are loaned crosslinguistically. In English , the longest possible initial cluster is three consonants, as in split /ˈsplɪt/ , strudel /ˈstruːdəl/ , strengths /ˈstrɛŋkθs/ , and "squirrel" /ˈskwɪrəl/ , all beginning with /s/ or /ʃ/ , containing /p/ , /t/ , or /k/ , and ending with /l/ , /r/ , or /w/ ;

7777-536: The languages of the world. Consonant clusters have a tendency to fall under patterns such as the sonority sequencing principle (SSP); the closer a consonant in a cluster is to the syllable's vowel, the more sonorous the consonant is. Among the most common types of clusters are initial stop- liquid sequences, such as in Thai (e.g. /pʰl/ , /tr/ , and /kl/ ). Other common ones include initial stop-approximant (e.g. Thai /kw/ ) and initial fricative-liquid (e.g. English /sl/ ) sequences. More rare are sequences which defy

7878-522: The latter type include the following: Digraphs may also be composed of vowels. Some letters ⟨a, e, o⟩ are preferred for the first position, others for the second ⟨i, u⟩ . The latter have allographs ⟨y, w⟩ in English orthography . In Serbo-Croatian : Note that in the Cyrillic orthography , those sounds are represented by single letters (љ, њ, џ). In Czech and Slovak : In Danish and Norwegian : In Norwegian , several sounds can be represented only by

7979-405: The longest possible final cluster is five consonants, as in angsts ( /ˈæŋksts/ ), though this is rare (perhaps owing to being derived from a recent German loanword). However, the /k/ in angsts may also be considered epenthetic ; for many speakers , nasal-sibilant sequences in the coda require insertion of a voiceless stop homorganic to the nasal. For speakers without this feature, the word

8080-537: The major cities, the difference between / ç / and / ʃ / has been completely wiped away and are now pronounced the same. In Catalan : In Dutch : In French : See also French phonology . In German : In Hungarian : In Italian : In Manx Gaelic , ⟨ch⟩ represents /χ/ , but ⟨çh⟩ represents /tʃ/ . In Polish : In Portuguese : In Spanish : In Welsh : The digraphs listed above represent distinct phonemes and are treated as separate letters for collation purposes. On

8181-634: The medial. These four segments are grouped into two slightly different components: In many languages of the Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area , such as Chinese , the syllable structure is expanded to include an additional, optional medial segment located between the onset (often termed the initial in this context) and the rime. The medial is normally a semivowel , but reconstructions of Old Chinese generally include liquid medials ( /r/ in modern reconstructions, /l/ in older versions), and many reconstructions of Middle Chinese include

8282-402: The modern Chinese varieties, use the terms "final" and "rime" interchangeably. In historical Chinese phonology , however, the distinction between "final" (including the medial) and "rime" (not including the medial) is important in understanding the rime dictionaries and rime tables that form the primary sources for Middle Chinese , and as a result most authors distinguish the two according to

8383-671: The most common syllabic consonants are sonorants like [l] , [r] , [m] , [n] or [ŋ] , as in English bott le , ch ur ch (in rhotic accents), rhyth m , butt on and lock ' n key . However, English allows syllabic obstruents in a few para-verbal onomatopoeic utterances such as shh (used to command silence) and psst (used to attract attention). All of these have been analyzed as phonemically syllabic. Obstruent-only syllables also occur phonetically in some prosodic situations when unstressed vowels elide between obstruents, as in potato [pʰˈteɪɾəʊ] and today [tʰˈdeɪ] , which do not change in their number of syllables despite losing

8484-511: The name has stuck. Ancient Greek also had the "diphthongs" listed above although their pronunciation in ancient times is disputed. In addition, Ancient Greek also used the letter γ combined with a velar stop to produce the following digraphs: Tsakonian has a few additional digraphs: In addition, palatal consonants are indicated with the vowel letter ι , which is, however, largely predictable. When /n/ and /l/ are not palatalized before ι , they are written νν and λλ . In Bactrian ,

8585-404: The nucleus and coda may each branch into multiple phonemes . The limit for the number of phonemes which may be contained in each varies by language. For example, Japanese and most Sino-Tibetan languages do not have consonant clusters at the beginning or end of syllables, whereas many Eastern European languages can have more than two consonants at the beginning or end of the syllable. In English,

8686-429: The nucleus of rhotic English church , there is debate over whether these nuclei are consonants or vowels. Languages of the northwest coast of North America, including Salishan , Wakashan and Chinookan languages, allow stop consonants and voiceless fricatives as syllables at the phonemic level, in even the most careful enunciation. An example is Chinook [ɬtʰpʰt͡ʃʰkʰtʰ] 'those two women are coming this way out of

8787-473: The nucleus, and the coda (literally 'tail') is the sound or sounds that follow the nucleus. They are sometimes collectively known as the shell . The term rime covers the nucleus plus coda. In the one-syllable English word cat , the nucleus is a (the sound that can be shouted or sung on its own), the onset c , the coda t , and the rime at . This syllable can be abstracted as a consonant-vowel-consonant syllable, abbreviated CVC . Languages vary greatly in

8888-411: The onset may have up to three consonants, and the coda four. Rime and rhyme are variants of the same word, but the rarer form rime is sometimes used to mean specifically syllable rime to differentiate it from the concept of poetic rhyme . This distinction is not made by some linguists and does not appear in most dictionaries. A heavy syllable is generally one with a branching rime , i.e. it

8989-559: The other hand, the digraphs ⟨ mh ⟩ , ⟨ nh ⟩ , and the trigraph ⟨ ngh ⟩ , which stand for voiceless consonants but occur only at the beginning of words as a result of the nasal mutation , are not treated as separate letters, and thus are not included in the alphabet. Daighi tongiong pingim , a transcription system used for Taiwanese Hokkien , includes or that represents /ə/ ( mid central vowel ) or /o/ ( close-mid back rounded vowel ), as well as other digraphs. In Yoruba , ⟨gb⟩

9090-431: The palatal nasal. Some consonant clusters originate from the loss of a vowel in between two consonants, usually (but not always) due to vowel reduction caused by lack of stress. This is also the origin of most consonant clusters in English, some of which go back to Proto-Indo-European times. For example, ⟨glow⟩ comes from Proto-Germanic *glo-, which in turn comes from Proto-Indo-European *gʰel-ó, where *gʰel-

9191-426: The restrictions on the sounds making up the onset, nucleus and coda of a syllable, according to what is termed a language's phonotactics . Although every syllable has supra-segmental features, these are usually ignored if not semantically relevant, e.g. in tonal languages . In the syllable structure of Sinitic languages , the onset is replaced with an initial, and a semivowel or liquid forms another segment, called

9292-636: The rule that two fricatives may not appear adjacently word-initially. Some English words, including thrash, three, throat, and throw, start with the voiceless dental fricative /θ/, the liquid /r/, or the /r/ cluster (/θ/+/r/). This cluster example in Proto-Germanic has a counterpart in which /θ/ was followed by /l/. In early North and West Germanic, the /l/ cluster disappeared. This suggests that clusters are affected as words are loaned to other languages. The examples show that every language has syllable preference based on syllable structure and segment harmony of

9393-537: The sequence sh could mean either ša or saha. However, digraphs are used for the aspirated and murmured consonants (those spelled with h- digraphs in Latin transcription) in languages of South Asia such as Urdu that are written in the Arabic script by a special form of the letter h , which is used only for aspiration digraphs, as can be seen with the following connecting (kh) and non-connecting (ḍh) consonants: In

9494-552: The sequences ⟨дж⟩ and ⟨дз⟩ do occur (mainly in loanwords) but are pronounced as combinations of an implosive (sometimes treated as an affricate) and a fricative; implosives are treated as allophones of the plosive /d̪/ and so those sequences are not considered to be digraphs. Cyrillic has few digraphs unless it is used to write non-Slavic languages, especially Caucasian languages . Because vowels are not generally written, digraphs are rare in abjads like Arabic. For example, if sh were used for š, then

9595-411: The syllable is considered left-branching, i.e. onset and nucleus group below a higher-level unit, called a "body" or "core". This contrasts with the coda. The rime or rhyme of a syllable consists of a nucleus and an optional coda . It is the part of the syllable used in most poetic rhymes , and the part that is lengthened or stressed when a person elongates or stresses a word in speech. The rime

9696-446: The syllable. English allows very complicated syllables; syllables may begin with up to three consonants (as in strength ), and occasionally end with as many as four (as in angsts , pronounced [æŋsts]). Many other languages are much more restricted; Japanese , for example, only allows /ɴ/ and a chroneme in a coda, and theoretically has no consonant clusters at all, as the onset is composed of at most one consonant. The linking of

9797-491: The syllables are. Digraph (orthography) A digraph (from Ancient Greek δίς ( dís )  'double' and γράφω ( gráphō )  'to write') or digram is a pair of characters used in the orthography of a language to write either a single phoneme (distinct sound), or a sequence of phonemes that does not correspond to the normal values of the two characters combined. Some digraphs represent phonemes that cannot be represented with

9898-433: The typical theory of syllable structure, the general structure of a syllable (σ) consists of three segments. These segments are grouped into two components: The syllable is usually considered right-branching, i.e. nucleus and coda are grouped together as a "rime" and are only distinguished at the second level. The nucleus is usually the vowel in the middle of a syllable. The onset is the sound or sounds occurring before

9999-481: The verb λαμβάνω lambánō "take". The noun uses the root λαβ- , which appears in the aorist tense; the present tense stem λαμβάν- is formed by adding a nasal infix ⟨ μ ⟩ ⟨m⟩ before the β b and a suffix -αν -an at the end. In the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA), the fullstop ⟨ . ⟩ marks syllable breaks, as in

10100-490: The water'. Linguists have analyzed this situation in various ways, some arguing that such syllables have no nucleus at all and some arguing that the concept of "syllable" cannot clearly be applied at all to these languages. Other examples: In Bagemihl's survey of previous analyses, he finds that the Bella Coola word /t͡sʼktskʷt͡sʼ/ 'he arrived' would have been parsed into 0, 2, 3, 5, or 6 syllables depending on which analysis

10201-412: The word "astronomical" ⟨ /ˌæs.trə.ˈnɒm.ɪk.əl/ ⟩. In practice, however, IPA transcription is typically divided into words by spaces, and often these spaces are also understood to be syllable breaks. In addition, the stress mark ⟨ ˈ ⟩ is placed immediately before a stressed syllable, and when the stressed syllable is in the middle of a word, in practice, the stress mark also marks

#323676