The tremoloa / ˈ t r eɪ m oʊ l oʊ w ə / , plural tremoloas , is a stringed instrument belonging to the fretless zither family . It was produced in United States in response to the rapid increase in popularity of Hawaiian music during the 1920s, and continued to be produced until the 1950s. Musical collective Broken Social Scene features the instrument in "Tremoloa Debut." The instrument is also featured sporadically on The Magnetic Fields' album 69 Love Songs .
58-454: The tremoloa simulates the tonal effects of the Hawaiian steel guitar by passing a weighted roller stabilized by a swinging lever termed an arm, along a melody string . Following, moving the roller after plucking creates tremolo , an effect which gave rise to its name. Additionally, the tremoloa possesses four chords (C, G, F, and D major), to strum out the harmony . The patent for
116-406: A change is perceived) depends on the tone's frequency content. Below 500 Hz, the jnd is about 3 Hz for sine waves, and 1 Hz for complex tones; above 1000 Hz, the jnd for sine waves is about 0.6% (about 10 cents ). The jnd is typically tested by playing two tones in quick succession with the listener asked if there was a difference in their pitches. The jnd becomes smaller if
174-439: A different combination of these frequencies, as well as harmonics and overtones. The sound waves of the different frequencies overlap and combine, and the balance of these amplitudes is a major factor in the characteristic sound of each instrument. William Sethares wrote that just intonation and the western equal tempered scale are related to the harmonic spectra /timbre of many western instruments in an analogous way that
232-480: A harsh, even and aggressive tone). On electric guitar and electric piano, performers can change the timbre using effects units and graphic equalizers . Tone quality and tone color are synonyms for timbre , as well as the " texture attributed to a single instrument". However, the word texture can also refer to the type of music, such as multiple, interweaving melody lines versus a singable melody accompanied by subordinate chords . Hermann von Helmholtz used
290-443: A non-transposing instrument like a violin calls B ♭ ." Pitches are labeled using: For example, one might refer to the A above middle C as a′ , A 4 , or 440 Hz . In standard Western equal temperament , the notion of pitch is insensitive to "spelling": the description "G 4 double sharp" refers to the same pitch as A 4 ; in other temperaments, these may be distinct pitches. Human perception of musical intervals
348-402: A real number, p , as follows. This creates a linear pitch space in which octaves have size 12, semitones (the distance between adjacent keys on the piano keyboard) have size 1, and A440 is assigned the number 69. (See Frequencies of notes .) Distance in this space corresponds to musical intervals as understood by musicians. An equal-tempered semitone is subdivided into 100 cents . The system
406-451: A rough analogy with visual brightness . Timbre researchers consider brightness to be one of the perceptually strongest distinctions between sounds and formalize it acoustically as an indication of the amount of high-frequency content in a sound, using a measure such as the spectral centroid . Pitch (music) Pitch is a perceptual property that allows sounds to be ordered on a frequency -related scale , or more commonly, pitch
464-414: A sound is also greatly affected by the following aspects of its envelope : attack time and characteristics, decay, sustain, release ( ADSR envelope ) and transients . Thus these are all common controls on professional synthesizers . For instance, if one takes away the attack from the sound of a piano or trumpet, it becomes more difficult to identify the sound correctly, since the sound of the hammer hitting
522-438: A sound or note a musical instrument produces is sometimes described in terms of a sum of a number of distinct frequencies . The lowest frequency is called the fundamental frequency , and the pitch it produces is used to name the note, but the fundamental frequency is not always the dominant frequency. The dominant frequency is the frequency that is most heard, and it is always a multiple of the fundamental frequency. For example,
580-413: A stimulus. The precise way this temporal structure helps code for pitch at higher levels is still debated, but the processing seems to be based on an autocorrelation of action potentials in the auditory nerve. However, it has long been noted that a neural mechanism that may accomplish a delay—a necessary operation of a true autocorrelation—has not been found. At least one model shows that a temporal delay
638-410: A tonal sound is a musical sound that has a definite pitch, such as pressing a key on a piano; a sound with a noiselike character would be white noise , the sound similar to that produced when a radio is not tuned to a station. Erickson gives a table of subjective experiences and related physical phenomena based on Schouten's five attributes: See also Psychoacoustic evidence below. The richness of
SECTION 10
#1732902532577696-538: Is almost entirely determined by how quickly the sound wave is making the air vibrate and has almost nothing to do with the intensity, or amplitude , of the wave. That is, "high" pitch means very rapid oscillation, and "low" pitch corresponds to slower oscillation. Despite that, the idiom relating vertical height to sound pitch is shared by most languages. At least in English, it is just one of many deep conceptual metaphors that involve up/down. The exact etymological history of
754-465: Is approximately logarithmic with respect to fundamental frequency : the perceived interval between the pitches "A220" and "A440" is the same as the perceived interval between the pitches A440 and A880 . Motivated by this logarithmic perception, music theorists sometimes represent pitches using a numerical scale based on the logarithm of fundamental frequency. For example, one can adopt the widely used MIDI standard to map fundamental frequency, f , to
812-563: Is closely related to frequency, but the two are not equivalent. Frequency is an objective, scientific attribute which can be measured. Pitch is the subjective perception of a sound wave by the individual person, which cannot be directly measured. However, this does not necessarily mean that people will not agree on which notes are higher and lower. The oscillations of sound waves can often be characterized in terms of frequency . Pitches are usually associated with, and thus quantified as, frequencies (in cycles per second, or hertz), by comparing
870-479: Is flexible enough to include "microtones" not found on standard piano keyboards. For example, the pitch halfway between C (60) and C ♯ (61) can be labeled 60.5. The following table shows frequencies in Hertz for notes in various octaves, named according to the "German method" of octave nomenclature : The relative pitches of individual notes in a scale may be determined by one of a number of tuning systems . In
928-537: Is one where a listener can possibly (or relatively easily) discern the pitch. Sounds with definite pitch have harmonic frequency spectra or close to harmonic spectra. A sound generated on any instrument produces many modes of vibration that occur simultaneously. A listener hears numerous frequencies at once. The vibration with the lowest frequency is called the fundamental frequency ; the other frequencies are overtones . Harmonics are an important class of overtones with frequencies that are integer multiples of
986-494: Is still possible for two sounds of indefinite pitch to clearly be higher or lower than one another. For instance, a snare drum sounds higher pitched than a bass drum though both have indefinite pitch, because its sound contains higher frequencies. In other words, it is possible and often easy to roughly discern the relative pitches of two sounds of indefinite pitch, but sounds of indefinite pitch do not neatly correspond to any specific pitch. A pitch standard (also concert pitch )
1044-475: Is the conventional pitch reference that musical instruments in a group are tuned to for a performance. Concert pitch may vary from ensemble to ensemble, and has varied widely over musical history. Standard pitch is a more widely accepted convention. The A above middle C is usually set at 440 Hz (often written as "A = 440 Hz " or sometimes "A440"), although other frequencies, such as 442 Hz, are also often used as variants. Another standard pitch,
1102-408: Is the quality that makes it possible to judge sounds as "higher" and "lower" in the sense associated with musical melodies . Pitch is a major auditory attribute of musical tones , along with duration , loudness , and timbre . Pitch may be quantified as a frequency , but pitch is not a purely objective physical property; it is a subjective psychoacoustical attribute of sound. Historically,
1160-477: Is unnecessary to produce an autocorrelation model of pitch perception, appealing to phase shifts between cochlear filters; however, earlier work has shown that certain sounds with a prominent peak in their autocorrelation function do not elicit a corresponding pitch percept, and that certain sounds without a peak in their autocorrelation function nevertheless elicit a pitch. To be a more complete model, autocorrelation must therefore apply to signals that represent
1218-538: The octave doubles the frequency of a note; for example, an octave above A440 is 880 Hz. If however the first overtone is sharp due to inharmonicity , as in the extremes of the piano, tuners resort to octave stretching . In atonal , twelve tone , or musical set theory , a "pitch" is a specific frequency while a pitch class is all the octaves of a frequency. In many analytic discussions of atonal and post-tonal music, pitches are named with integers because of octave and enharmonic equivalency (for example, in
SECTION 20
#17329025325771276-623: The reciprocal of the time interval between repeating similar events in the sound waveform. The pitch of complex tones can be ambiguous, meaning that two or more different pitches can be perceived, depending upon the observer. When the actual fundamental frequency can be precisely determined through physical measurement, it may differ from the perceived pitch because of overtones , also known as upper partials, harmonic or otherwise. A complex tone composed of two sine waves of 1000 and 1200 Hz may sometimes be heard as up to three pitches: two spectral pitches at 1000 and 1200 Hz, derived from
1334-466: The tritone paradox , but most notably the Shepard scale , where a continuous or discrete sequence of specially formed tones can be made to sound as if the sequence continues ascending or descending forever. Not all musical instruments make notes with a clear pitch. The unpitched percussion instruments (a class of percussion instruments ) do not produce particular pitches. A sound or note of definite pitch
1392-517: The A above middle C to 432 Hz or 435 Hz when performing repertoire from the Romantic era. Transposing instruments have their origin in the variety of pitch standards. In modern times, they conventionally have their parts transposed into different keys from voices and other instruments (and even from each other). As a result, musicians need a way to refer to a particular pitch in an unambiguous manner when talking to each other. For example,
1450-589: The German Klangfarbe ( tone color ), and John Tyndall proposed an English translation, clangtint , but both terms were disapproved of by Alexander Ellis , who also discredits register and color for their pre-existing English meanings. Determined by its frequency composition, the sound of a musical instrument may be described with words such as bright , dark , warm , harsh , and other terms. There are also colors of noise , such as pink and white . In visual representations of sound, timbre corresponds to
1508-435: The acoustic waveform of the above instruments must exist which are invariant with respect to the above variables". However, Robert Erickson argues that there are few regularities and they do not explain our "...powers of recognition and identification." He suggests borrowing the concept of subjective constancy from studies of vision and visual perception . Psychoacoustic experiments from the 1960s onwards tried to elucidate
1566-526: The apparent pitch shifts were not significantly different from pitch‐matching errors. When averaged, the remaining shifts followed the directions of Stevens's curves but were small (2% or less by frequency, i.e. not more than a semitone). Theories of pitch perception try to explain how the physical sound and specific physiology of the auditory system work together to yield the experience of pitch. In general, pitch perception theories can be divided into place coding and temporal coding . Place theory holds that
1624-410: The dominant frequency for the transverse flute is double the fundamental frequency. Other significant frequencies are called overtones of the fundamental frequency, which may include harmonics and partials . Harmonics are whole number multiples of the fundamental frequency, such as ×2, ×3, ×4, etc. Partials are other overtones. There are also sometimes subharmonics at whole number divisions of
1682-545: The early twentieth century. Norman Del Mar describes the following passage from the Scherzo movement of his Sixth Symphony , as "a seven-bar link to the trio consisting of an extension in diminuendo of the repeated As… though now rising in a succession of piled octaves which moreover leap-frog with Cs added to the As. The lower octaves then drop away and only the Cs remain so as to dovetail with
1740-443: The first oboe phrase of the trio." During these bars, Mahler passes the repeated notes through a gamut of instrumental colors, mixed and single: starting with horns and pizzicato strings, progressing through trumpet, clarinet, flute, piccolo and finally, oboe: (See also Klangfarbenmelodie .) In rock music from the late 1960s to the 2000s, the timbre of specific sounds is important to a song. For example, in heavy metal music ,
1798-448: The frequency spectrum, although it also depends upon the sound pressure and the temporal characteristics of the sound". Many commentators have attempted to decompose timbre into component attributes. For example, J. F. Schouten (1968, 42) describes the "elusive attributes of timbre" as "determined by at least five major acoustic parameters", which Robert Erickson finds, "scaled to the concerns of much contemporary music": An example of
Tremoloa - Misplaced Pages Continue
1856-414: The fundamental frequency. Most instruments produce harmonic sounds, but many instruments produce partials and inharmonic tones, such as cymbals and other indefinite-pitched instruments. When the tuning note in an orchestra or concert band is played, the sound is a combination of 440 Hz, 880 Hz, 1320 Hz, 1760 Hz and so on. Each instrument in the orchestra or concert band produces
1914-456: The fundamental. Whether or not the higher frequencies are integer multiples, they are collectively called the partials , referring to the different parts that make up the total spectrum. A sound or note of indefinite pitch is one that a listener finds impossible or relatively difficult to identify as to pitch. Sounds with indefinite pitch do not have harmonic spectra or have altered harmonic spectra—a characteristic known as inharmonicity . It
1972-526: The inharmonic timbre of the Thai renat (a xylophone-like instrument) is related to the seven-tone near-equal tempered pelog scale in which they are tuned. Similarly, the inharmonic spectra of Balinese metallophones combined with harmonic instruments such as the stringed rebab or the voice, are related to the five-note near-equal tempered slendro scale commonly found in Indonesian gamelan music. The timbre of
2030-435: The low and middle frequency ranges. Moreover, there is some evidence that some non-human primates lack auditory cortex responses to pitch despite having clear tonotopic maps in auditory cortex, showing that tonotopic place codes are not sufficient for pitch responses. Temporal theories offer an alternative that appeals to the temporal structure of action potentials, mostly the phase-lock of action potentials to frequencies in
2088-427: The most common type of clarinet or trumpet , when playing a note written in their part as C, sounds a pitch that is called B ♭ on a non-transposing instrument like a violin (which indicates that at one time these wind instruments played at a standard pitch a tone lower than violin pitch). To refer to that pitch unambiguously, a musician calls it concert B ♭ , meaning, "the pitch that someone playing
2146-406: The musical sense of high and low pitch is still unclear. There is evidence that humans do actually perceive that the source of a sound is slightly higher or lower in vertical space when the sound frequency is increased or reduced. In most cases, the pitch of complex sounds such as speech and musical notes corresponds very nearly to the repetition rate of periodic or nearly-periodic sounds, or to
2204-441: The nature of timbre. One method involves playing pairs of sounds to listeners, then using a multidimensional scaling algorithm to aggregate their dissimilarity judgments into a timbre space. The most consistent outcomes from such experiments are that brightness or spectral energy distribution, and the bite , or rate and synchronicity and rise time, of the attack are important factors. The concept of tristimulus originates in
2262-488: The nineteenth and the first decades of the twentieth centuries, has been credited with elevating further the role of timbre: "To a marked degree the music of Debussy elevates timbre to an unprecedented structural status; already in Prélude à l'après-midi d'un faune the color of flute and harp functions referentially". Mahler 's approach to orchestration illustrates the increasing role of differentiated timbres in music of
2320-399: The nineteenth century. For example, Wagner's "Sleep motif" from Act 3 of his opera Die Walküre , features a descending chromatic scale that passes through a gamut of orchestral timbres. First the woodwind (flute, followed by oboe), then the massed sound of strings with the violins carrying the melody, and finally the brass (French horns). Debussy , who composed during the last decades of
2378-506: The output of the cochlea , as via auditory-nerve interspike-interval histograms. Some theories of pitch perception hold that pitch has inherent octave ambiguities, and therefore is best decomposed into a pitch chroma , a periodic value around the octave, like the note names in Western music—and a pitch height , which may be ambiguous, that indicates the octave the pitch is in. The just-noticeable difference (jnd) (the threshold at which
Tremoloa - Misplaced Pages Continue
2436-420: The perception of pitch is determined by the place of maximum excitation on the basilar membrane . A place code, taking advantage of the tonotopy in the auditory system, must be in effect for the perception of high frequencies, since neurons have an upper limit on how fast they can phase-lock their action potentials . However, a purely place-based theory cannot account for the accuracy of pitch perception in
2494-440: The perception of timbre include frequency spectrum and envelope . Singers and instrumental musicians can change the timbre of the music they are singing/playing by using different singing or playing techniques. For example, a violinist can use different bowing styles or play on different parts of the string to obtain different timbres (e.g., playing sul tasto produces a light, airy timbre, whereas playing sul ponticello produces
2552-408: The physical frequencies of the pure tones, and the combination tone at 200 Hz, corresponding to the repetition rate of the waveform. In a situation like this, the percept at 200 Hz is commonly referred to as the missing fundamental , which is often the greatest common divisor of the frequencies present. Pitch depends to a lesser degree on the sound pressure level (loudness, volume) of
2610-447: The relative weight of the first harmonic; the second tristimulus measures the relative weight of the second, third, and fourth harmonics taken together; and the third tristimulus measures the relative weight of all the remaining harmonics: However, more evidence, studies and applications would be needed regarding this type of representation, in order to validate it. The term "brightness" is also used in discussions of sound timbres, in
2668-444: The same category (e.g., an oboe and a clarinet , both woodwind instruments ). In simple terms, timbre is what makes a particular musical instrument or human voice have a different sound from another, even when they play or sing the same note. For instance, it is the difference in sound between a guitar and a piano playing the same note at the same volume. Both instruments can sound equally tuned in relation to each other as they play
2726-409: The same note, and while playing at the same amplitude level each instrument will still sound distinctively with its own unique tone color. Experienced musicians are able to distinguish between different instruments of the same type based on their varied timbres, even if those instruments are playing notes at the same fundamental pitch and loudness. The physical characteristics of sound that determine
2784-449: The shape of the image, while loudness corresponds to brightness; pitch corresponds to the y-shift of the spectrogram. The Acoustical Society of America (ASA) Acoustical Terminology definition 12.09 of timbre describes it as "that attribute of auditory sensation which enables a listener to judge that two nonidentical sounds, similarly presented and having the same loudness and pitch , are dissimilar", adding, "Timbre depends primarily upon
2842-404: The so-called Baroque pitch , has been set in the 20th century as A = 415 Hz—approximately an equal-tempered semitone lower than A440 to facilitate transposition. The Classical pitch can be set to either 427 Hz (about halfway between A415 and A440) or 430 Hz (also between A415 and A440 but slightly sharper than the quarter tone). And ensembles specializing in authentic performance set
2900-568: The sonic impact of the heavily amplified, heavily distorted power chord played on electric guitar through very loud guitar amplifiers and rows of speaker cabinets is an essential part of the style's musical identity. Often, listeners can identify an instrument, even at different pitches and loudness, in different environments, and with different players. In the case of the clarinet , acoustic analysis shows waveforms irregular enough to suggest three instruments rather than one. David Luce suggests that this implies that "[C]ertain strong regularities in
2958-490: The sounds being assessed against sounds with pure tones (ones with periodic , sinusoidal waveforms). Complex and aperiodic sound waves can often be assigned a pitch by this method. According to the American National Standards Institute , pitch is the auditory attribute of sound allowing those sounds to be ordered on a scale from low to high. Since pitch is such a close proxy for frequency, it
SECTION 50
#17329025325773016-399: The strings or the first blast of the player's lips on the trumpet mouthpiece are highly characteristic of those instruments. The envelope is the overall amplitude structure of a sound. Instrumental timbre played an increasing role in the practice of orchestration during the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries. Berlioz and Wagner made significant contributions to its development during
3074-437: The study of pitch and pitch perception has been a central problem in psychoacoustics, and has been instrumental in forming and testing theories of sound representation, processing, and perception in the auditory system. Pitch is an auditory sensation in which a listener assigns musical tones to relative positions on a musical scale based primarily on their perception of the frequency of vibration ( audio frequency ). Pitch
3132-489: The tone, especially at frequencies below 1,000 Hz and above 2,000 Hz. The pitch of lower tones gets lower as sound pressure increases. For instance, a tone of 200 Hz that is very loud seems one semitone lower in pitch than if it is just barely audible. Above 2,000 Hz, the pitch gets higher as the sound gets louder. These results were obtained in the pioneering works by S. Stevens and W. Snow. Later investigations, e.g. by A. Cohen, have shown that in most cases
3190-576: The tremoloa was granted in 1932 to Harold Finney and John H. Large. This article relating to zithers is a stub . You can help Misplaced Pages by expanding it . Timbre In music, timbre ( / ˈ t æ m b ər , ˈ t ɪ m -, ˈ t æ̃ -/ ), also known as tone color or tone quality (from psychoacoustics ), is the perceived sound quality of a musical note , sound or tone . Timbre distinguishes different types of sound production, such as choir voices and musical instruments. It also enables listeners to distinguish different instruments in
3248-399: The two tones are played simultaneously as the listener is then able to discern beat frequencies . The total number of perceptible pitch steps in the human hearing range is about 1,400; the total number of notes in the equal-tempered scale, from 16 to 16,000 Hz, is 120. The relative perception of pitch can be fooled, resulting in aural illusions . There are several of these, such as
3306-475: The west, the twelve-note chromatic scale is the most common method of organization, with equal temperament now the most widely used method of tuning that scale. In it, the pitch ratio between any two successive notes of the scale is exactly the twelfth root of two (or about 1.05946). In well-tempered systems (as used in the time of Johann Sebastian Bach , for example), different methods of musical tuning were used. In almost all of these systems interval of
3364-409: The world of color, describing the way three primary colors can be mixed together to create a given color. By analogy, the musical tristimulus measures the mixture of harmonics in a given sound, grouped into three sections. It is basically a proposal of reducing a huge number of sound partials, which can amount to dozens or hundreds in some cases, down to only three values. The first tristimulus measures
#576423