Jalapa Mazatec is a Mazatecan language . An estimate from 1990 suggested it was spoken by 15,000 people, one-third of whom are monolingual, in 13 villages in the vicinity of the town of San Felipe Jalapa de Díaz in the Tuxtepec District of the Mexican state of Oaxaca . A 2016 study, published in 2019, estimated the Mazatec dialects to have 220,000 speakers. Egland (1978) found 73% intelligibility with Huautla , the prestige variety of Mazatec. Literacy in Jalapa is taught alongside Spanish in local schools.
39-656: Jalapa Mazatec root words are primarily monosyllabic, and the intricate inflectional system is largely subsyllablic (Silverman 1994). Jalapa Mazatec syllables are maximally C C G V . However, vowels distinguish several phonations , and like all Mazatec languages, Jalapa has tone . Jalapa roots distinguish three tones, low ˩ , mid ˧ , and high ˥ . In morphologically complex situations, combinations of these may form short (or perhaps mid-length) vowels with contour tones : ˩˧, ˧˥, ˥˧, ˧˩, ˥˩, ˩˥˩ have been recorded. The simple tones are contrasted in /ʃá/ (/ʃa˥/) "work", /ʃā/ (/ʃa˧/) "puma", /ʃà/ (/ʃa˩/) "mould". In much of
78-517: A /ⁿʔ/ . Prenasalized stops may be distinguished from post-oralized or post-stopped nasals (orally released nasals), such as the [mᵇ nᵈ ɲᶡ ŋᶢ] of Acehnese and similar sounds (including voiceless [mᵖ] ) in many dialects of Chinese. (At least in the Chinese case, nasalization, in some dialects, continues in a reduced degree to the vowel, indicating that the consonant is partially denasalized , rather than actually having an oral release.) No language
117-592: A nasal and an obstruent (or occasionally a non-nasal sonorant ) that behave phonologically like single consonants . The primary reason for considering them to be single consonants, rather than clusters as in English finger or member , lies in their behaviour; however, there may also be phonetic correlates which distinguish prenasalized consonants from clusters. Because of the additional difficulty in both articulation and timing, prenasalized fricatives and sonorants are not as common as prenasalized stops or affricates, and
156-522: A prenasalized stop in his name, as does the capital of Chad , N'Djamena (African prenasalized stops are often written with apostrophes in Latin script transcription although this may sometimes indicate syllabic nasals instead). The sound [ g͡b] can also be found in approximately 90 languages in Africa. In Southern Min languages, such as Teochew , prenasalized stops are also found. The prenasalized stops in
195-540: Is not marked in this article apart from this one table: Jalapa consonants distinguish (prenasalized) voiced, tenuis , and aspirated plosives , as well as voiceless, voiced, and glottalized sonorants . There is also a flap , /ɾ/ , which only occurs in one morpheme, the clitic = /ɾa/ "probably". In addition, the consonants /p/ , /pʰ/ , /l/ are found in Spanish loan words . The labial velars /ʍ w w̰/ become bilabial [ɸ β β̰] before front vowels : [ɸǣ] "it
234-421: Is a sibilant and C is a tenuis plosive or affricate. Root word A root (also known as root word or radical ) is the core of a word that is irreducible into more meaningful elements. In morphology , a root is a morphologically simple unit which can be left bare or to which a prefix or a suffix can attach. The root word is the primary lexical unit of a word , and of a word family (this root
273-409: Is believed to contrast the two types of consonant, which are distinguished primarily by a difference in timing (a brief nasal followed by longer stop, as opposed to a longer nasal followed by brief stop). The Bantu languages are famous for their prenasalized stops (the "nt" in "Bantu" is an example), but similar sounds occur across Africa and around the world. Ghana 's politician Kwame Nkrumah had
312-412: Is finished" vs. [ʍā] "John", etc. In the same position, the stop /kʷʰ/ is realized as a heterorganic velar-bilabial affricate [kɸ]. Phonetically aspirated fricatives do not occur before creaky vowels, while aspirated stops do. Therefore, Silverman et al. (1994) treats them as fricative-/h/ clusters. Silverman (1994:126) remarks that voiced stops are prenasalized in intervocalic position, but later on
351-458: Is morphologically similar to the production of frequentative (iterative) verbs in Latin , for example: Consider also Rabbinic Hebrew ת-ר-מ √t-r-m ‘donate, contribute’ (Mishnah: T’rumoth 1:2: ‘separate priestly dues’), which derives from Biblical Hebrew תרומה t'rūmå ‘contribution’, whose root is ר-ו-מ √r-w-m ‘raise’; cf. Rabbinic Hebrew ת-ר-ע √t-r-' ‘sound
390-419: Is one of only three languages reported to have a contrast between prenasalized consonants and their corresponding clusters, along with Fula and Selayarese , although the nature of this contrast is debated. For example, Sri Lankan Malay has been in contact with Sinhala a long time and has also developed prenasalized stops. The spectrograms on the right show the word gaambar with a prenasalized stop and
429-807: Is spoken by people in Papua New Guinea who have similar phonologies in their languages, voiced consonants are prenasalized. For example, the preposition bilong (from English belong ) is pronounced [ᵐbiloŋ] by many Melanesians . The prenasalization behaves as a phonetic detail of voicing, rather than a separate segment. Prenasalized stops are also found in Australia. The Eastern Arrernte language has both prenasalized stops and prestopped nasals , but does not have any other word-initial consonant clusters . Compare [mʷarə] "good", [ᵐpʷaɻə] "make", [ᵖmʷaɻə] " coolamon ". When unambiguous, prenasalized consonants may simply be transcribed e.g. ⟨ mb ⟩. In
SECTION 10
#1732870076325468-437: Is then called the base word), which carries aspects of semantic content and cannot be reduced into smaller constituents. Content words in nearly all languages contain, and may consist only of, root morphemes . However, sometimes the term "root" is also used to describe the word without its inflectional endings, but with its lexical endings in place. For example, chatters has the inflectional root or lemma chatter , but
507-677: The Arabic language : Similar cases occur in Hebrew , for example Israeli Hebrew מ-ק-מ √m-q-m ‘locate’, which derives from Biblical Hebrew מקום måqom ‘place’, whose root is ק-ו-מ √q-w-m ‘stand’. A recent example introduced by the Academy of the Hebrew Language is מדרוג midrúg ‘rating’, from מדרג midrág , whose root is ד-ר-ג √d-r-g ‘grade’." According to Ghil'ad Zuckermann , "this process
546-641: The Hmong–Mien language family of Southern China and Southeast Asia. In dialects of northern Japan , standard voiced stops are prenasalized, and voiceless stops are voiced. For example, /itiɡo/ "strawberry" is [it̠͡ɕiɡo] in most of the south, but [id̠͡ʑɨᵑɡo] in much of the north. Prenasalized stops are also reconstructed for Old Japanese . In Greek the orthographic sequences μπ, ντ γκ and γγ are often pronounced as prenasalized voiced stops [ᵐb] , [ⁿd] , and [ᵑɡ] , respectively, especially in formal speech and among older speakers. Among younger Athenian speakers
585-514: The beginning of a word, but it does not allow other consonant sequences. Thus the prenasalized stops behave like ordinary consonants. In some Oceanic languages , prenasalisation of voiced consonants depends on the environment. For example, in Raga , b and d are prenasalized when the preceding consonant is nasal ( noⁿda "ours"), but not elsewhere ( gida "us"). Uneapa has prenasalization word-medially, but not word-initially ( goᵐbu "yam"). When Tok Pisin
624-410: The category-neutral approach, data from English indicates that the same underlying root appears as a noun and a verb - with or without overt morphology. In Hebrew , the majority of roots consist of segmental consonants √CCC. Arad (2003) describes that the consonantal root is turned into a word due to pattern morphology. Thereby, the root is turned into a verb when put into a verbal environment where
663-437: The end; a fully creaky consonant; or the creak may extend into the following vowel. Aspirated consonants do not occur before breathy vowels, and glottalized consonants only occur before modally voiced vowels. Nasal consonants only occur before nasal vowels. Voiced plosives are prenasalized in intervocalic position. Consonant clusters include NC, where N is a nasal and C is a voiceless plosive or affricate, and SC, where S
702-488: The first 40% and then have modal voice, so that for example /mæ̤˧˩/ may be pronounced [mæ̤̃˧˩] or [mæ̥̃æ̃˧˩] . Similarly, creaky vowels tend to confine their creakiness to the first part of the vowel, often with glottal closure before modal voice: /sḭ˥/ as [sḭi˥] or [sḭʔi˥] . Jalapa is unique among the Mazatec languages in distinguishing breathy vowels. These arose through the contraction of Proto-Mazatecan disyllables of
741-464: The form CVhV, where C was voiced and the two vowels were the same. When the two syllables carried different tones, these contracted into a contour. For example, proto-Mazatec *ntʲa˩hu˩ "stone" became /ndʲo̤˩/ (through a presumed intermediate *ndʲo˩ho˩ ); *ntʃe˨he˦ "thief" became /ndʒæ̤˩˧/ ; and *ntu˩hwi˩˧ "your soap" became /ndɨ̤ː˩˧/ . Similar contractions occurred with CVʔV disyllables to produce creaky vowels, but creaky vowels already existed in
780-436: The forms derived from the abstract consonantal roots , a major Hebrew phonetics concept ג-ד-ל ( g-d-l ) related to ideas of largeness: g a d o l and gd o l a (masculine and feminine forms of the adjective "big"), g a d a l "he grew", hi gd i l "he magnified" and ma gd e l et "magnifier", along with many other words such as g o d e l "size" and mi gd a l "tower". Roots and reconstructed roots can become
819-400: The head bears the "v" feature (the pattern). Consider the root √š-m-n (ש-מ-נ). Although all words vary semantically, the general meaning of a greasy, fatty material can be attributed to the root. Furthermore, Arad states that there are two types of languages in terms of root interpretation. In languages like English, the root is assigned one interpretation whereas in languages like Hebrew,
SECTION 20
#1732870076325858-420: The lexical root chat . Inflectional roots are often called stems . A root, or a root morpheme , in the stricter sense, may be thought of as a monomorphemic stem. The traditional definition allows roots to be either free morphemes or bound morphemes . Root morphemes are the building blocks for affixation and compounds . However, in polysynthetic languages with very high levels of inflectional morphology,
897-534: The literature, these are written with the numerals 1 (low), 2 (mid), and 3 (high). Jalapa utilizes whistled speech , where each simple or contour tone is given a whistle pulse. Jalapa Mazatec distinguishes five vowel qualities, discounting phonation: /i/ , /æ/ , /a/ , /o/ , /u/ . Phonations are modal voice , breathy voice , and creaky voice ; all phonations may also occur with the five nasal vowels : Breathy vowels may have strong breathy voicing throughout their length. However, typically they are voiceless for
936-483: The prenasalization often disappears and in fast speech the voiced stop may be replaced by a fricative. The Guaraní language has a set of prenasalized stops which are alternate allophonically with simple nasal continuants; they appear only within a word, to the left of a stressed vowel that is oral. The Indo-Aryan languages Sinhala and Dhivehi have prenasalized stops. Sinhala script has prenasalized versions of / g / , / ʥ /, / ɖ / , / d̪ / and / b / . Sinhala
975-686: The presence of the former implies the latter. Only three languages ( Sinhala , Fula , Selayarese ) have been reported to have a contrast between prenasalized consonants ( C) and their corresponding clusters (NC). In most languages, when a prenasalized consonant is described as "voiceless", it is only the oral portion that is voiceless, and the nasal portion is modally voiced . Thus, a language may have "voiced" [ᵐb ⁿd ᶯɖ ᶮɟ ᵑɡ ᶰɢ] and "voiceless" [ᵐp ⁿt ᶯʈ ᶮc ᵑk ᶰq] . However, in some Southern Min (including Taiwanese ) dialects, voiced consonants are preceded by voiceless prenasalization: [ᵐ̥b ⁿ̥d ⁿ̥ɺ ᵑ̊ɡ] . Yeyi has prenasalized ejectives . Adzera has
1014-403: The proto-language. Jalapa also has a phonemic distinction of unclear nature that has been suggested to be " ballisticity ". However, it lacks the characteristics of ballistic syllables in other Otomanguean languages. The only consistent distinction Silverman et al. (1994) were able to measure was one of vowel length , with vowels of the alleged ballistic syllables being two-thirds the length of
1053-713: The root -rupt , which only appears in other related prefixd forms (such as disrupt , corrupt , rupture , etc.). The form -rupt cannot occur on its own. Examples of ( consonantal roots ) which are related but distinct to the concept developed here are formed prototypically by three (as few as two and as many as five) consonants. Speakers may derive and develop new words (morphosyntactically distinct, i.e. with different parts of speech) by using non-concatenative morphological strategies: inserting different vowels . Unlike 'root' here, these cannot occur on their own without modification; as such these are never actually observed in speech and may be termed 'abstract'. For example, in Hebrew ,
1092-401: The root ampli- . In the former case, the root can occur on its own freely. In the latter, it requires modification via affixation to be used as a free form. English has minimal use of morphological strategies such as affixation and features a tendency to have words that are identical to their roots. However, such forms as in Spanish exist in English such as interrupt , which may arguably contain
1131-594: The root can form multiple interpretations depending on its environment. This occurrence suggests a difference in language acquisition between these two languages. English speakers would need to learn two roots in order to understand two different words whereas Hebrew speakers would learn one root for two or more words. Alexiadou and Lohndal (2017) advance the claim that languages have a typological scale when it comes to roots and their meanings and state that Greek lies in between Hebrew and English. Prenasalization Prenasalized consonants are phonetic sequences of
1170-480: The roots' vowels, by adding or removing the long vowels a , i , u , e and o . (Notice that Arabic does not have the vowels e and o .) In addition, secondary roots can be created by prefixing ( m− , t− ), infixing ( −t− ), or suffixing ( −i , and several others). There is no rule in these languages on how many secondary roots can be derived from a single root; some roots have few, but other roots have many, not all of which are necessarily in current use. Consider
1209-498: The same page states that they are prenasalized in initial position. With voiced plosives, the nasalization is two-thirds the duration of the consonant. It is not clear if they ever appear without prenasalization. Voiceless nasals are voiced for the last quarter of their duration. Glottalized sonorants are variable in their production. The may occur as a glottal stop followed by a modally voiced sonorant, [ʔm] , [ʔj] , etc.; an initially creaky voiced sonorant switching to modal voice by
Jalapa Mazatec - Misplaced Pages Continue
1248-468: The syllabification of sambal sam.bal. An example of the unitary behavior of prenasalized stops is provided by Fijian . In this language, as in many in Melanesia and also reconstructed for Proto-Oceanic , there is a series of voiceless stops, [p, t, k] , and a series of prenasalized stops, [ᵐb, ⁿd, ᵑɡ] , but there are no simple voiced stops, [b, d, ɡ] . In addition, Fijian allows prenasalized stops at
1287-551: The syntactic environment. The ways in which these roots gain lexical category are discussed in Distributed Morphology and the Exoskeletal Model . Theories adopting a category-neutral approach have not, as of 2020, reached a consensus about whether these roots contain a semantic type but no argument structure, neither semantic type nor argument structure, or both semantic type and argument structure. In support of
1326-517: The term "root" is generally synonymous with "free morpheme". Many such languages have a very restricted number of morphemes that can stand alone as a word: Yup'ik , for instance, has no more than two thousand. The root is conventionally indicated using the mathematical symbol √; for instance, the Sanskrit root " √bhū- " means the root " bhū- ". English verb form running contains the root run . The Spanish superlative adjective amplísimo contains
1365-434: The tools of etymology . Secondary roots are roots with changes in them, producing a new word with a slightly different meaning. In English, a rough equivalent would be to see conductor as a secondary root formed from the root to conduct . In abjad languages, the most familiar of which are Arabic and Hebrew , in which families of secondary roots are fundamental to the language, secondary roots are created by changes in
1404-460: The trumpet, blow the horn’, from Biblical Hebrew תרועה t'rū`å ‘shout, cry, loud sound, trumpet-call’, in turn from ר-ו-ע √r-w-`." and it describes the suffix. Decompositional generative frameworks suggest that roots hold little grammatical information and can be considered "category-neutral". Category-neutral roots are roots without any inherent lexical category but with some conceptual content that becomes evident depending on
1443-619: The vernacular readings of Southern Min languages evolved not from the different Middle Chinese initials and thus are historically different from the voiced obstruents found in Wu and Xiang languages. Prenasalized consonants are widely utilized in the Loloish languages of the Lolo–Burmese family, such as Yi and Naxi . The following table illustrates the prenasalized consonants in northern Yi. The prenasalized stops also occur in several branches of
1482-402: The vowels of the productive open class of nouns, with a slight increase in pitch. They may reflect the original short vowels of proto-Mazatec, as opposed to the vowels of morphologically complex monosyllabic nouns of modern Jalapa Mazatec. If so, Jalapa would have a three-way length distinction, as doubly long vowels are also found in morphologically complex situations. Note that this distinction
1521-514: The word sambal with a sequence of nasal+voiced stop, yet not prenasalized. The difference in the length of the [m] part is clearly visible. The nasal in the prenasalized word is much shorter than the nasal in the other word. This phonetic information is complemented by phonological evidence: The first vowel in gaambar is lengthened, which only happens in open syllables in Sri Lanka Malay. The syllabification of gaambar must be gaa.mbar then, and
#324675