Construction grammar (often abbreviated CxG ) is a family of theories within the field of cognitive linguistics which posit that constructions , or learned pairings of linguistic patterns with meanings, are the fundamental building blocks of human language. Constructions include words ( aardvark , avocado ), morphemes ( anti- , -ing ), fixed expressions and idioms ( by and large , jog X's memory ), and abstract grammatical rules such as the passive voice ( The cat was hit by a car ) or the ditransitive ( Mary gave Alex the ball ). Any linguistic pattern is considered to be a construction as long as some aspect of its form or its meaning cannot be predicted from its component parts, or from other constructions that are recognized to exist. In construction grammar, every utterance is understood to be a combination of multiple different constructions, which together specify its precise meaning and form.
49-457: CXG may refer to: Construction grammar (CxG), a family of theories in the field of cognitive and evolutionary linguistics Crazy Ex-Girlfriend , a 2015 American romantic musical comedy-drama television series Topics referred to by the same term [REDACTED] This disambiguation page lists articles associated with the title CXG . If an internal link led you here, you may wish to change
98-837: A computational model for large scale practical usage in Natural Language Processing frameworks but interest in construction grammar has been shown by more traditional computational linguists as a contrast to the current boom in more opaque deep learning models. This is largely due to the representational convenience of CxG models and their potential to integrate with current tokenizers as a perceptual layer for further processing in neurally inspired models. Approaches to integrate constructional grammar with existing Natural Language Processing frameworks include hand-built feature sets and templates and used computational models to identify their prevalence in text collections, but some suggestions for more emergent models have been proposed, e.g. in
147-503: A group of lexemes. Combinatorial constructions include both inflectional and derivational constructions. SBCG is both formal and generative; while cognitive-functional grammarians have often opposed their standards and practices to those of formal, generative grammarians, there is in fact no incompatibility between a formal, generative approach and a rich, broad-coverage, functionally based grammar. It simply happens that many formal, generative theories are descriptively inadequate grammars. SBCG
196-491: A learned association of form and prototypical meaning in order to set up the constructions said to form the basic units of syntax. Lexical item In lexicography , a lexical item is a single word, a part of a word, or a chain of words ( catena ) that forms the basic elements of a language's lexicon (≈ vocabulary). Examples are cat , traffic light , take care of , by the way , and it's raining cats and dogs . Lexical items can be generally understood to convey
245-546: A model merely of idioms or infrequently used, minor patterns. As the pairing of the syntactic construction and its prototypical meaning are learned in early childhood, children should initially learn the basic constructions with their prototypical semantics, that is, 'agent of action' for the subject in the SV relation, 'affected object of agent's action' for the direct object term in VO, and 'recipient in transfer of possession of object' for
294-463: A neutralization of semantic distinctions. For instance, in a detailed discussion of the dissociation of grammatical case-roles from semantics, Talmy Givon lists the multiple semantic roles of subjects and direct objects in English. As these phenomena are well-established, some linguists propose that core grammatical relations be excluded from CxG as they are not constructions, leaving the theory to be
343-464: A particle verb construction, which is a single lexical item. The two words remain a catena even as shifting changes their order of appearance. The following trees illustrate polywords: The component words of the polywords (in red) are continuous in the vertical dimension and are therefore catenae. They cannot, however, be construed as constituents since they do not form complete subtrees. The following trees illustrate idioms: The fixed words constituting
392-420: A process which is comparable to natural selection in species or the formation of natural constructions such as nests made by social insects . Constructions correspond to replicators or memes in memetics and other cultural replicator theories. It is argued that construction grammar is not an original model of cultural evolution, but for essential part the same as memetics . Construction grammar
441-584: A single meaning, much as a lexeme , but are not limited to single words. Lexical items are like semes in that they are "natural units" translating between languages, or in learning a new language. In this last sense, it is sometimes said that language consists of grammaticalized lexis, and not lexicalized grammar. The entire store of lexical items in a language is called its lexis . Lexical items composed of more than one word are also sometimes called lexical chunks , gambits , lexical phrases , lexicalized stems , or speech formulae . The term polyword listemes
490-439: A uniform mechanism for parsing and production. Moreover, it has been demonstrated through robotic experiments that FCG grammars can be grounded in embodiment and sensorimotor experiences. FCG integrates many notions from contemporary computational linguistics such as feature structures and unification-based language processing. Constructions are considered bidirectional and hence usable both for parsing and production. Processing
539-438: A word, whereas many other lexical items consist of parts of one or more words or of multiple words in their entirety. A basic question in this area concerns the form-meaning correspondence. Many multi-word lexical items cannot be construed as constituents in syntax in any sense. But if they are not constituents, then how does one classify them? A relatively recent development in the field of syntax envisages lexical items stored in
SECTION 10
#1733093490422588-494: Is a dispute between the advocates of construction grammar and memetics , an evolutionary approach which adheres to the Darwinian view of language and culture. Advocates of construction grammar argue that memetics takes the perspective of intelligent design to cultural evolution while construction grammar rejects human free will in language construction; but, according to memetician Susan Blackmore , this makes construction grammar
637-405: Is also sometimes used. Common types of lexical items/chunks include: An associated concept is that of noun-modifier semantic relations , wherein certain word pairings have a standard interpretation. For example, the phrase cold virus is generally understood to refer to the virus that causes a cold, rather than to a virus that is cold. Many lexical items are either a whole word or part of
686-406: Is argued that children pay close attention to each utterance they hear, and gradually make generalizations based on the utterances they have heard. Because constructions are learned, they are expected to vary considerably across different languages. In construction grammar, as in general semiotics , the grammatical construction is a pairing of form and content. The formal aspect of a construction
735-438: Is associated with concepts from cognitive linguistics that aim to show in various ways how human rational and creative behaviour is automatic and not planned. Construction grammar was first developed in the 1980s by linguists such as Charles Fillmore , Paul Kay , and George Lakoff , in order to analyze idioms and fixed expressions. Lakoff's 1977 paper "Linguistic Gestalts" put forward an early version of CxG, arguing that
784-673: Is described by a lexeme or construction. Recent SBCG works have expanded on the lexicalist model of idiomatically combining expressions sketched out in Sag 2012. The type of construction grammar associated with linguists like Goldberg and Lakoff looks mainly at the external relations of constructions and the structure of constructional networks. In terms of form and function, this type of construction grammar puts psychological plausibility as its highest desideratum. It emphasizes experimental results and parallels with general cognitive psychology. It also draws on certain principles of cognitive linguistics. In
833-529: Is flexible in the sense that it can even cope with partially ungrammatical or incomplete sentences. FCG is called 'fluid' because it acknowledges the premise that language users constantly change and update their grammars. The research on FCG is conducted at Sony CSL Paris and the AI Lab at the Vrije Universiteit Brussel . Most of the above approaches to construction grammar have not been implemented as
882-464: Is generative in a way that prevailing syntax-centered theories are not: its mechanisms are intended to represent all of the patterns of a given language, including idiomatic ones; there is no 'core' grammar in SBCG. SBCG a licensing-based theory, as opposed to one that freely generates syntactic combinations and uses general principles to bar illicit ones: a word, lexeme or phrase is well formed if and only if it
931-814: Is often recognized, and that they are best understood as multi-word, partially filled constructions. Construction grammar rejects the idea that there is a sharp dichotomy between lexical items , which are arbitrary and specific, and grammatical rules, which are completely general. Instead, CxG posits that there are linguistic patterns at every level of generality and specificity: from individual words, to partially filled constructions (e.g. drive X crazy ), to fully abstract rules (e.g. subject–auxiliary inversion ). All of these patterns are recognized as constructions. In contrast to theories that posit an innate universal grammar for all languages, construction grammar holds that speakers learn constructions inductively as they are exposed to them, using general cognitive processes. It
980-565: Is stored in the taxonomies: Because construction grammar does not operate with surface derivations from underlying structures, it adheres to functionalist linguist Dwight Bolinger 's principle of no synonymy , on which Adele Goldberg elaborates in her book. This means that construction grammarians argue, for instance, that active and passive versions of the same proposition are not derived from an underlying structure, but are instances of two different constructions. As constructions are pairings of form and meaning, active and passive versions of
1029-413: Is that conceptual semantics is primary to the degree that form mirrors, or is motivated by, content. Langacker argues that even abstract grammatical units like part-of-speech classes are semantically motivated and involve certain conceptualizations. William A. Croft 's radical construction grammar is designed for typological purposes and takes into account cross-linguistic factors. It deals mainly with
SECTION 20
#17330934904221078-496: Is that the content of all linguistic signs involves mental simulations and is ultimately dependent on basic image schemas of the kind advocated by Mark Johnson and George Lakoff, and so ECG aligns itself with cognitive linguistics. Like construction grammar, embodied construction grammar makes use of a unification-based model of representation. A non-technical introduction to the NTL theory behind embodied construction grammar as well as
1127-462: Is the sign, with subtypes word, lexeme and phrase. The inclusion of phrase within the canon of signs marks a major departure from traditional syntactic thinking. In SBCG, phrasal signs are licensed by correspondence to the mother of some licit construct of the grammar. A construct is a local tree with signs at its nodes. Combinatorial constructions define classes of constructs. Lexical class constructions describe combinatoric and other properties common to
1176-543: Is typically described as a syntactic template, but the form covers more than just syntax, as it also involves phonological aspects, such as prosody and intonation . The content covers semantic as well as pragmatic meaning. The semantic meaning of a grammatical construction is made up of conceptual structures postulated in cognitive semantics : image-schemas , frames, conceptual metaphors, conceptual metonymies, prototypes of various kinds, mental spaces, and bindings across these (called "blends"). Pragmatics just becomes
1225-539: The ditransitive schema [S V IO DO] is said to express semantic content X CAUSES Y TO RECEIVE Z, just like kill means X CAUSES Y TO DIE. In construction grammar, a grammatical construction, regardless of its formal or semantic complexity and make up, is a pairing of form and meaning. Thus words and word classes may be regarded as instances of constructions. Indeed, construction grammarians argue that all pairings of form and meaning are constructions, including phrase structures, idioms , words and even morphemes . Unlike
1274-503: The 2023 Georgetown University Roundtable on Linguistics. Esa Itkonen , who defends humanistic linguistics and opposes Darwinian linguistics, questions the originality of the work of Adele Goldberg, Michael Tomasello, Gilles Fauconnier , William Croft and George Lakoff. According to Itkonen, construction grammarians have appropriated old ideas in linguistics adding some false claims. For example, construction type and conceptual blending correspond to analogy and blend , respectively, in
1323-455: The Goldbergian strand, constructions interact with each other in a network via four inheritance relations: polysemy link, subpart link, metaphorical extension, and finally instance link. Sometimes, Ronald Langacker's cognitive grammar framework is described as a type of construction grammar. Cognitive grammar deals mainly with the semantic content of constructions, and its central argument
1372-532: The VP construction but contain no phrasal information in themselves. In the mid-2000s, several of the developers of BCG, including Charles Fillmore, Paul Kay, Ivan Sag and Laura Michaelis, collaborated in an effort to improve the formal rigor of BCG and clarify its representational conventions. The result was Sign Based Construction Grammar (SBCG). SBCG is based on a multiple-inheritance hierarchy of typed feature structures. The most important type of feature structure in SBCG
1421-462: The children, Ninio also found that the pattern of subjects, direct objects and indirect objects in mothers’ speech does not provide the required prototypical semantics for the construction to be established. Adele Goldberg and her associates had previously reported similar negative results concerning the pattern of direct objects in parental speech. These findings are a blow to the CxG theory that relies on
1470-605: The cognitive semantics of communication—the modern version of the old Ross -Lakoff performative hypothesis from the 1960s. The form and content are symbolically linked in the sense advocated by Langacker . Thus a construction is treated like a sign in which all structural aspects are integrated parts and not distributed over different modules as they are in the componential model. Consequentially, not only constructions that are lexically fixed, like many idioms, but also more abstract ones like argument structure schemata, are pairings of form and conventionalized meaning. For instance,
1519-548: The componential model, construction grammar denies any strict distinction between the two and proposes a syntax–lexicon continuum . The argument goes that words and complex constructions are both pairs of form and meaning and differ only in internal symbolic complexity. Instead of being discrete modules and thus subject to very different processes they form the extremes of a continuum (from regular to idiosyncratic): syntax > subcategorization frame > idiom > morphology > syntactic category > word/ lexicon (these are
CXG - Misplaced Pages Continue
1568-588: The formal aspects of constructions and makes use of a unification-based framework for description of syntax, not unlike head-driven phrase structure grammar . Its proponents/developers include Charles Fillmore, Paul Kay, Laura Michaelis , and to a certain extent Ivan Sag . Immanent within BCG works like Fillmore and Kay 1995 and Michaelis and Ruppenhofer 2001 is the notion that phrasal representations—embedding relations—should not be used to represent combinatoric properties of lexemes or lexeme classes. For example, BCG abandons
1617-624: The idea that form is semantically motivated. Embodied construction grammar (ECG), which is being developed by the Neural Theory of Language (NTL) group at ICSI, UC Berkeley, and the University of Hawaiʻi, particularly including Benjamin Bergen and Nancy Chang, adopts the basic constructionist definition of a grammatical construction, but emphasizes the relation of constructional semantic content to embodiment and sensorimotor experiences. A central claim
1666-449: The idiom (in red) build a catena each time. Note that your is not part of the idiom in the first tree (tree a) because the possessor is variable, e.g. He is pulling my/her/his/someone's/etc. leg . An important caveat concerning idiom catenae is that they can be broken up in the syntax, e.g. Your leg is being pulled . The claim, however, is that these lexical items are stored as catenae in the lexicon; they do not always appear as catenae in
1715-463: The indirect-object in VI. Anat Ninio examined the speech of a large sample of young English-speaking children and found that they do not in fact learn the syntactic patterns with the prototypical semantics claimed to be associated with them, or with any single semantics. The major reason is that such pairings are not consistently modelled for them in parental speech. Examining the maternal speech addressed to
1764-688: The internal structure of constructions. Radical construction grammar is totally non- reductionist , and Croft argues that constructions are not derived from their parts, but that the parts are derived from the constructions they appear in. Thus, in radical construction grammar, constructions are linked to Gestalts . Radical construction grammar rejects the idea that syntactic categories, roles, and relations are universal and argues that they are not only language-specific, but also construction specific. Thus, there are no universals that make reference to formal categories, since formal categories are language- and construction-specific. The only universals are to be found in
1813-562: The lexicon as catenae , whereby a given catena may or may not be a constituent. In syntax, a catena is any element or combination of elements (words or parts of words) that are continuous in the vertical dimension, that is, in the hierarchy of words. The elements form a catena insofar as they are linked together by dependencies. Some dependency grammar trees containing multiple-word lexical items that are catenae but not constituents are now produced. The following trees illustrate phrasal verbs: The verb and particle (in red) in each case constitute
1862-509: The link to point directly to the intended article. Retrieved from " https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=CXG&oldid=989040319 " Category : Disambiguation pages Hidden categories: Short description is different from Wikidata All article disambiguation pages All disambiguation pages Construction grammar Advocates of construction grammar argue that language and culture are not designed by people, but are 'emergent' or automatically constructed in
1911-515: The meaning of an expression was not simply a function of the meanings of its parts. Instead, he suggested, constructions themselves must have meanings. Another early study was "There-Constructions," which appeared as Case Study 3 in George Lakoff's Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things . It argued that the meaning of the whole was not a function of the meanings of the parts, that odd grammatical properties of Deictic There-constructions followed from
1960-406: The patterns concerning the mapping of meaning onto form. Radical construction grammar rejects the notion of syntactic relations altogether and replaces them with semantic relations. Like Goldbergian/Lakovian construction grammar and cognitive grammar, radical construction grammar is closely related to cognitive linguistics, and like cognitive grammar, radical construction grammar appears to be based on
2009-421: The pragmatic meaning of the construction, and that variations on the central construction could be seen as simple extensions using form-meaning pairs of the central construction. Fillmore et al.'s (1988) paper on the English let alone construction was a second classic. These two papers propelled cognitive linguists into the study of CxG. Since the late 1990s there has been a shift towards a general preference for
CXG - Misplaced Pages Continue
2058-541: The proverbial expression The bigger they come, the harder they fall . Construction grammarians point out that this is not merely a fixed phrase; the Correlative Conditional is a general pattern ( The Xer, the Yer ) with "slots" that can be filled by almost any comparative phrase (e.g. The more you think about it, the less you understand ). Advocates of CxG argue these kinds of idiosyncratic patterns are more common than
2107-405: The same as memetics. Lastly, the most basic syntactic patterns of English, namely the core grammatical relations subject-verb, verb object and verb-indirect object, are counter-evidence for the very concept of constructions as pairings of linguistic patterns with meanings. Instead of the postulated form-meaning pairing, core grammatical relations possess a wide variability of semantics, exhibiting
2156-411: The same proposition are not synonymous, but display differences in content: in this case the pragmatic content. As mentioned above, Construction grammar is a "family" of theories rather than one unified theory. There are a number of formalized Construction grammar frameworks. Some of these are: Berkeley Construction Grammar (BCG: formerly also called simply Construction Grammar in upper case) focuses on
2205-477: The theory itself and a variety of applications can be found in Jerome Feldman's From Molecule to Metaphor: A Neural Theory of Language (MIT Press, 2006). Fluid construction grammar (FCG) was designed by Luc Steels and his collaborators for doing experiments on the origins and evolution of language . FCG is a fully operational and computationally implemented formalism for construction grammars and proposes
2254-644: The traditional practice of using non-branching domination (NP over N' over N) to describe undetermined nominals that function as NPs, instead introducing a determination construction that requires ('asks for') a non-maximal nominal sister and a lexical 'maximality' feature for which plural and mass nouns are unmarked. BCG also offers a unification-based representation of 'argument structure' patterns as abstract verbal lexeme entries ('linking constructions'). These linking constructions include transitive, oblique goal and passive constructions. These constructions describe classes of verbs that combine with phrasal constructions like
2303-447: The traditional terms; construction grammars use a different terminology). In construction grammar, the grammar of a language is made up of taxonomic networks of families of constructions, which are based on the same principles as those of the conceptual categories known from cognitive linguistics , such as inheritance, prototypicality, extensions, and multiple parenting. Four different models are proposed in relation to how information
2352-447: The usage-based model. The shift towards the usage-based approach in construction grammar has inspired the development of several corpus -based methodologies of constructional analysis (for example, collostructional analysis ). One of the most distinctive features of CxG is its use of multi-word expressions and phrasal patterns as the building blocks of syntactic analysis. One example is the Correlative Conditional construction, found in
2401-423: The works of William Dwight Whitney , Leonard Bloomfield , Charles Hockett , and others. At the same time, the claim made by construction grammarians, that their research represents a continuation of Saussurean linguistics, has been considered misleading. German philologist Elisabeth Leiss regards construction grammar as regress, linking it with the 19th century social darwinism of August Schleicher . There
#421578