Misplaced Pages

Analysis of covariance

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.

Analysis of covariance ( ANCOVA ) is a general linear model that blends ANOVA and regression . ANCOVA evaluates whether the means of a dependent variable (DV) are equal across levels of one or more categorical independent variables (IV) and across one or more continuous variables. For example, the categorical variable(s) might describe treatment and the continuous variable(s) might be covariates (CV)'s, typically nuisance variables; or vice versa. Mathematically, ANCOVA decomposes the variance in the DV into variance explained by the CV(s), variance explained by the categorical IV, and residual variance. Intuitively, ANCOVA can be thought of as 'adjusting' the DV by the group means of the CV(s).

#612387

72-427: The ANCOVA model assumes a linear relationship between the response (DV) and covariate (CV): y i j = μ + τ i + B ( x i j − x ¯ ) + ϵ i j . {\displaystyle y_{ij}=\mu +\tau _{i}+\mathrm {B} (x_{ij}-{\overline {x}})+\epsilon _{ij}.} In this equation,

144-444: A τ i = 0 ) . {\displaystyle \left(\sum _{i}^{a}\tau _{i}=0\right).} The standard assumptions of the linear regression model are also assumed to hold, as discussed below. ANCOVA can be used to increase statistical power (the probability a significant difference is found between groups when one exists) by reducing the within-group error variance . In order to understand this, it

216-419: A Monte Carlo simulation method that works more generally. Once again, we return to the assumption of the distribution of D n {\displaystyle D_{n}} and the definition of T n {\displaystyle T_{n}} . Suppose we have fixed values of the sample size, variability and effect size, and wish to compute power. We can adopt this process: 1. Generate

288-434: A replication crisis . However, excessive demands for power could be connected to wasted resources and ethical problems, for example the use of a large number of animal test subjects when a smaller number would have been sufficient. It could also induce researchers trying to seek funding to overstate their expected effect sizes, or avoid looking for more subtle interaction effects that cannot be easily detected. Power analysis

360-404: A cost in resources. How increased sample size translates to higher power is a measure of the efficiency of the test – for example, the sample size required for a given power. The statistical power of a hypothesis test has an impact on the interpretation of its results. Not finding a result with a more powerful study is stronger evidence against the effect existing than the same finding with

432-480: A different (in this case, larger) sample size. Alternatively, multiple under-powered studies can still be useful, if appropriately combined through a meta-analysis . Many statistical analyses involve the estimation of several unknown quantities. In simple cases, all but one of these quantities are nuisance parameters . In this setting, the only relevant power pertains to the single quantity that will undergo formal statistical inference. In some settings, particularly if

504-476: A frequentist hypothesis testing framework, this is done by calculating a test statistic (such as a t-statistic ) for the dataset, which has a known theoretical probability distribution if there is no difference (the so called null hypothesis). If the actual value calculated on the sample is sufficiently unlikely to arise under the null hypothesis, we say we identified a statistically significant effect. The threshold for significance can be set small to ensure there

576-420: A given experimental or analytic setup, and so power is higher. The nature of the sample underlies the information being used in the test. This will usually involve the sample size, and the sample variability, if that is not implicit in the definition of the effect size. More broadly, the precision with which the data are measured can also be an important factor (such as the statistical reliability ), as well as

648-489: A higher threshold of stringency to reject a hypothesis (such as with the Bonferroni method ), and so would reduce power. Alternatively, there may be different notions of power connected with how the different hypotheses are considered. "Complete power" demands that all true effects are detected across all of the hypotheses, which is a much stronger requirement than the "minimal power" of being able to find at least one true effect,

720-471: A large number of sets of D n {\displaystyle D_{n}} according to the null hypothesis, N ( 0 , σ D ) {\displaystyle N(0,\sigma _{D})} 2. Compute the resulting test statistic T n {\displaystyle T_{n}} for each set. 3. Compute the ( 1 − α ) {\displaystyle (1-\alpha )} th quantile of

792-477: A less powerful study. However, this is not completely conclusive. The effect may exist, but be smaller than what was looked for, meaning the study is in fact underpowered and the sample is thus unable to distinguish it from random chance. Many clinical trials , for instance, have low statistical power to detect differences in adverse effects of treatments, since such effects may only affect a few patients, even if this difference can be important . Conclusions about

SECTION 10

#1732891485613

864-510: A researcher perform a power analysis. An underpowered study is likely be inconclusive, failing to allow one to choose between hypotheses at the desired significance level, while an overpowered study will spend great expense on being able to report significant effects even if they are tiny and so practically meaningless. If a large number of underpowered studies are done and statistically significant results published , published findings are more likely false positives than true results, contributing to

936-787: A student-t distribution under H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}} , converging on to a standard normal distribution for large n . The estimated σ ^ D {\displaystyle {\hat {\sigma }}_{D}} will also converge on to its population value σ D {\displaystyle \sigma _{D}} Thus power can be approximated as B ( θ ) ≈ 1 − Φ ( 1.64 − θ σ D / n ) . {\displaystyle B(\theta )\approx 1-\Phi \left(1.64-{\frac {\theta }{\sigma _{D}/{\sqrt {n}}}}\right).} According to this formula,

1008-434: A type of power that might increase with an increasing number of hypotheses. Power analysis can either be done before ( a priori or prospective power analysis) or after ( post hoc or retrospective power analysis) data are collected. A priori power analysis is conducted prior to the research study, and is typically used in estimating sufficient sample sizes to achieve adequate power. Post-hoc analysis of "observed power"

1080-411: A type II error β is set as 1 - 0.8 = 0.2, while α, the probability of a type I error, is commonly set at 0.05. Some applications require much higher levels of power. Medical tests may be designed to minimise the number of false negatives (type II errors) produced by loosening the threshold of significance, raising the risk of obtaining a false positive (a type I error). The rationale

1152-414: A useful measure of how much a given experiment size can be expected to refine one's beliefs. A study with low power is unlikely to lead to a large change in beliefs. In addition, the concept of power is used to make comparisons between different statistical testing procedures: for example, between a parametric test and a nonparametric test of the same hypothesis. Tests may have the same size , and hence

1224-428: Is an estimate of the population variance and d = μ 1 − μ 2 {\displaystyle d=\mu _{1}-\mu _{2}} the to-be-detected difference in the mean values of both samples. This expression can be rearranged, implying for example that 80% power is obtained when looking for a difference in means that exceeds about 4 times the group-wise standard error of

1296-574: Is around 2, say, then we require for a power of B ( θ ) = 0.8 {\displaystyle B(\theta )=0.8} , a sample size n > 4 ( 1.64 − Φ − 1 ( 1 − 0.8 ) ) 2 ≈ 4 ( 1.64 + 0.84 ) 2 ≈ 24.6. {\displaystyle n>4\left(1.64-\Phi ^{-1}\left(1-0.8\right)\right)^{2}\approx 4\left(1.64+0.84\right)^{2}\approx 24.6.} Alternatively we can use

1368-411: Is conducted after a study has been completed, and uses the obtained sample size and effect size to determine what the power was in the study, assuming the effect size in the sample is equal to the effect size in the population. Whereas the utility of prospective power analysis in experimental design is universally accepted, post hoc power analysis is fundamentally flawed. Falling for the temptation to use

1440-619: Is defined as: T n = D ¯ n − μ 0 σ ^ D / n = D ¯ n − 0 σ ^ D / n , {\displaystyle T_{n}={\frac {{\bar {D}}_{n}-\mu _{0}}{{\hat {\sigma }}_{D}/{\sqrt {n}}}}={\frac {{\bar {D}}_{n}-0}{{\hat {\sigma }}_{D}/{\sqrt {n}}}},} where μ 0 {\displaystyle \mu _{0}}

1512-573: Is large, the t-distribution converges to the standard normal distribution (thus no longer involving n ) and so through use of the corresponding quantile function Φ − 1 {\displaystyle \Phi ^{-1}} , we obtain that the null should be rejected if T n > t α ≈ Φ − 1 ( 0.95 ) ≈ 1.64 . {\displaystyle T_{n}>t_{\alpha }\approx \Phi ^{-1}(0.95)\approx 1.64\,.} Now suppose that

SECTION 20

#1732891485613

1584-440: Is little chance of falsely detecting a non-existent effect. However, failing to identify a significant effect does not imply there was none. If we insist on being careful to avoid false positives, we may create false negatives instead. It may simply be too much to expect that we will be able to find satisfactorily strong evidence of a very subtle difference even if it exists. Statistical power is an attempt to quantify this issue. In

1656-544: Is natural to choose our null hypothesis to be that the expected mean difference is zero, i.e. H 0 : μ D = μ 0 = 0. {\displaystyle H_{0}:\mu _{D}=\mu _{0}=0.} For our one-sided test, the alternative hypothesis would be that there is a positive effect, corresponding to H 1 : μ D = θ > 0. {\displaystyle H_{1}:\mu _{D}=\theta >0.} The test statistic in this case

1728-511: Is necessary to understand the test used to evaluate differences between groups, the F-test . The F -test is computed by dividing the explained variance between groups (e.g., medical recovery differences) by the unexplained variance within the groups. Thus, If this value is larger than a critical value, we conclude that there is a significant difference between groups. Unexplained variance includes error variance (e.g., individual differences), as well as

1800-469: Is one minus the type II error probability and is also the sensitivity of the hypothesis testing procedure to detect a true effect. There is usually a trade-off between demanding more stringent tests (and so, smaller rejection regions) and trying to have a high probability of rejecting the null under the alternative hypothesis. Statistical power may also be extended to the case where multiple hypotheses are being tested based on an experiment or survey. It

1872-476: Is primarily a frequentist statistics tool. In Bayesian statistics , hypothesis testing of the type used in classical power analysis is not done. In the Bayesian framework, one updates his or her prior beliefs using the data obtained in a given study. In principle, a study that would be deemed underpowered from the perspective of hypothesis testing could still be used in such an updating process. However, power remains

1944-430: Is required to be clinically significant . An effect size can be a direct value of the quantity of interest (for example, a difference in mean of a particular size), or it can be a standardized measure that also accounts for the variability in the population (such as a difference in means expressed as a multiple of the standard deviation). If the researcher is looking for a larger effect, then it should be easier to find with

2016-432: Is that it is better to tell a healthy patient "we may have found something—let's test further," than to tell a diseased patient "all is well." Power analysis focuses on the correct rejection of a null hypothesis. Alternative concerns may however motivate an experiment, and so lead to different needs for sample size. In many contexts, the issue is less about deciding between hypotheses but rather with getting an estimate of

2088-825: Is the case here, as is typical, that power cannot be made equal to 1 except in the trivial case where α = 1 {\displaystyle \alpha =1} so the null is always rejected. We can invert B {\displaystyle B} to obtain required sample sizes: n > σ D θ ( 1.64 − Φ − 1 ( 1 − B ( θ ) ) ) . {\displaystyle {\sqrt {n}}>{\frac {\sigma _{D}}{\theta }}\left(1.64-\Phi ^{-1}\left(1-B(\theta )\right)\right).} Suppose θ = 1 {\displaystyle \theta =1} and we believe σ D {\displaystyle \sigma _{D}}

2160-565: Is the mean under the null so we substitute in 0, n is the sample size (number of subjects), D ¯ n {\displaystyle {\bar {D}}_{n}} is the sample mean of the difference D ¯ n = 1 n ∑ i = 1 n D i , {\displaystyle {\bar {D}}_{n}={\frac {1}{n}}\sum _{i=1}^{n}D_{i},} and σ ^ D {\displaystyle {\hat {\sigma }}_{D}}

2232-436: Is the probability of making a type II error (a false negative ) conditional on there being a true effect or association. Statistical testing uses data from samples to assess, or make inferences about, a statistical population . For example, we may measure the yields of samples of two varieties of a crop, and use a two sample test to assess whether the mean values of this yield differs between varieties. Under

Analysis of covariance - Misplaced Pages Continue

2304-451: Is the sample standard deviation of the difference. We can proceed according to our knowledge of statistical theory, though in practice for a standard case like this software will exist to compute more accurate answers. Thanks to t-test theory, we know this test statistic under the null hypothesis follows a Student t-distribution with n − 1 {\displaystyle n-1} degrees of freedom. If we wish to reject

2376-420: Is thought to show more evidence that the null hypothesis is actually true when the p -value is smaller, since the apparent power to detect an actual effect would be higher. In fact, a smaller p -value is properly understood to make the null hypothesis relatively less likely to be true. The following is an example that shows how to compute power for a randomized experiment: Suppose the goal of an experiment

2448-405: Is thus also common to refer to the power of a study , evaluating a scientific project in terms of its ability to answer the research questions they are seeking to answer. The main application of statistical power is "power analysis", a calculation of power usually done before an experiment is conducted using data from pilot studies or a literature review. Power analyses can be used to calculate

2520-421: Is to study the effect of a treatment on some quantity, and so we shall compare research subjects by measuring the quantity before and after the treatment, analyzing the data using a one-sided paired t-test , with a significance level threshold of 0.05. We are interested in being able to detect a positive change of size θ > 0 {\displaystyle \theta >0} . We first set up

2592-407: Is true. To make this more concrete, a typical statistical test would be based on a test statistic t calculated from the sampled data, which has a particular probability distribution under H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} . A desired significance level α would then define a corresponding "rejection region" (bounded by certain "critical values"), a set of values t

2664-461: Is unlikely to take if H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} was correct. If we reject H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} in favor of H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}} only when the sample t takes those values, we would be able to keep the probability of falsely rejecting H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} within our desired significance level. At

2736-423: The degrees of freedom . Accordingly, adding a covariate which accounts for very little variance in the dependent variable might actually reduce power. Statistical power In frequentist statistics , power is a measure of the ability of an experimental design and hypothesis testing setup to detect a particular effect if it is truly present. In typical use, it is a function of the test used (including

2808-433: The design of an experiment or observational study. Ultimately, these factors lead to an expected amount of sampling error . A smaller sampling error could be obtained by larger sample sizes from a less variability population, from more accurate measurements, or from more efficient experimental designs (for example, with the appropriate use of blocking ), and such smaller errors would lead to improved power, albeit usually at

2880-396: The i th level of the categorical IV), B {\displaystyle B} (the slope of the line) and ϵ i j {\displaystyle \epsilon _{ij}} (the associated unobserved error term for the j th observation in the i th group). Under this specification, the categorical treatment effects sum to zero ( ∑ i

2952-432: The power of the test is the probability that the test correctly rejects the null hypothesis ( H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} ) when the alternative hypothesis ( H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}} ) is true. It is commonly denoted by 1 − β {\displaystyle 1-\beta } , where β {\displaystyle \beta }

Analysis of covariance - Misplaced Pages Continue

3024-413: The probability of actual presence of an effect also should consider more things than a single test, especially as real world power is rarely close to 1. Indeed, although there are no formal standards for power, many researchers and funding bodies assess power using 0.80 (or 80%) as a standard for adequacy. This convention implies a four-to-one trade off between β -risk and α -risk, as the probability of

3096-516: The CV. However, even with the use of covariates, there are no statistical techniques that can equate unequal groups. Furthermore, the CV may be so intimately related to the categorical IV that removing the variance on the DV associated with the CV would remove considerable variance on the DV, rendering the results meaningless. There are several key assumptions that underlie the use of ANCOVA and affect interpretation of

3168-405: The DV over and above the other CV. One or the other should be removed since they are statistically redundant. Tested by Levene's test of equality of error variances. This is most important after adjustments have been made, but if you have it before adjustment you are likely to have it afterwards. To see if the CV significantly interacts with the categorical IV, run an ANCOVA model including both

3240-700: The DV, y i j {\displaystyle y_{ij}} is the jth observation under the ith categorical group; the CV, x i j {\displaystyle x_{ij}} is the j th observation of the covariate under the i th group. Variables in the model that are derived from the observed data are μ {\displaystyle \mu } (the grand mean) and x ¯ {\displaystyle {\overline {x}}} (the global mean for covariate x {\displaystyle x} ). The variables to be fitted are τ i {\displaystyle \tau _{i}} (the effect of

3312-504: The IV and the CVxIV interaction term. If the CVxIV interaction is significant, ANCOVA should not be performed. Instead, Green & Salkind suggest assessing group differences on the DV at particular levels of the CV. Also consider using a moderated regression analysis , treating the CV and its interaction as another IV. Alternatively, one could use mediation analyses to determine if the CV accounts for

3384-511: The IV's effect on the DV. If the CV×IV interaction is not significant, rerun the ANCOVA without the CV×IV interaction term. In this analysis, you need to use the adjusted means and adjusted mean squared error . The adjusted means (also referred to as least squares means, LS means, estimated marginal means, or EMM) refer to the group means after controlling for the influence of the CV on the DV. If there

3456-2652: The alternative hypothesis H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}} is true so μ D = θ {\displaystyle \mu _{D}=\theta } . Then, writing the power as a function of the effect size, B ( θ ) {\displaystyle B(\theta )} , we find the probability of T n {\displaystyle T_{n}} being above t α {\displaystyle t_{\alpha }} under H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}} . B ( θ ) ≈ Pr ( T n > 1.64   |   μ D = θ ) = Pr ( D ¯ n − 0 σ ^ D / n > 1.64   |   μ D = θ ) = 1 − Pr ( D ¯ n − 0 σ ^ D / n < 1.64   |   μ D = θ ) = 1 − Pr ( D ¯ n − θ σ ^ D / n < 1.64 − θ σ ^ D / n   |   μ D = θ ) {\displaystyle {\begin{aligned}B(\theta )&\approx \Pr \left(T_{n}>1.64~{\big |}~\mu _{D}=\theta \right)\\&=\Pr \left({\frac {{\bar {D}}_{n}-0}{{\hat {\sigma }}_{D}/{\sqrt {n}}}}>1.64~{\Big |}~\mu _{D}=\theta \right)\\&=1-\Pr \left({\frac {{\bar {D}}_{n}-0}{{\hat {\sigma }}_{D}/{\sqrt {n}}}}<1.64~{\Big |}~\mu _{D}=\theta \right)\\&=1-\Pr \left({\frac {{\bar {D}}_{n}-\theta }{{\hat {\sigma }}_{D}/{\sqrt {n}}}}<1.64-{\frac {\theta }{{\hat {\sigma }}_{D}/{\sqrt {n}}}}~{\Big |}~\mu _{D}=\theta \right)\\\end{aligned}}} D ¯ n − θ σ ^ D / n {\displaystyle {\frac {{\bar {D}}_{n}-\theta }{{\hat {\sigma }}_{D}/{\sqrt {n}}}}} again follows

3528-414: The case of the comparison of the two crop varieties, it enables us to answer questions like: Suppose we are conducting a hypothesis test. We define two hypotheses H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} the null hypothesis, and H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}} the alternative hypothesis. If we design the test such that α is the significance level - being

3600-402: The cost of requiring stronger assumptions. The magnitude of the effect of interest defines what is being looked for by the test. It can be the expected effect size if it exists, as a scientific hypothesis that the researcher has arrived at and wishes to test. Alternatively, in a more practical context it could be determined by the size the effect must be to be useful, for example that which

3672-430: The desired level of statistical significance ), the assumed distribution of the test (for example, the degree of variability, and sample size ), and the effect size of interest. High statistical power is related to low variability, large sample sizes, large effects being looked for, and less stringent requirements for statistical significance. More formally, in the case of a simple hypothesis test with two hypotheses,

SECTION 50

#1732891485613

3744-453: The error covariance matrix is diagonal. The residuals (error terms) should be normally distributed ϵ i j {\displaystyle \epsilon _{ij}} ~ N ( 0 , σ 2 ) {\displaystyle N(0,\sigma ^{2})} . The slopes of the different regression lines should be equivalent, i.e., regression lines should be parallel among groups. The fifth issue, concerning

3816-443: The following three aspects that can be potentially controlled by the practitioner: For a given test, the significance criterion determines the desired degree of rigor, specifying how unlikely it is for the null hypothesis of no effect to be rejected if it is in fact true. The most commonly used threshold is a probability of rejection of 0.05, though smaller values like 0.01 or 0.001 are sometimes used. This threshold then implies that

3888-406: The goals are more "exploratory", there may be a number of quantities of interest in the analysis. For example, in a multiple regression analysis we may include several covariates of potential interest. In situations such as this where several hypotheses are under consideration, it is common that the powers associated with the different hypotheses differ. For instance, in multiple regression analysis,

3960-422: The homogeneity of different treatment regression slopes is particularly important in evaluating the appropriateness of ANCOVA model. Also note that we only need the error terms to be normally distributed. In fact both the independent variable and the concomitant variables will not be normally distributed in most cases. If a CV is highly related to another CV (at a correlation of 0.5 or more), then it will not adjust

4032-691: The influence of other factors. Therefore, the influence of CVs is grouped in the denominator. When we control for the effect of CVs on the DV, we remove it from the denominator making F larger, thereby increasing our power to find a significant effect if one exists at all. Another use of ANCOVA is to adjust for preexisting differences in nonequivalent (intact) groups. This controversial application aims at correcting for initial group differences (prior to group assignment) that exists on DV among several intact groups. In this situation, participants cannot be made equal through random assignment, so CVs are used to adjust scores and make participants more similar than without

4104-403: The level of another factor. One can investigate the simple main effects using the same methods as in a factorial ANOVA . While the inclusion of a covariate into an ANOVA generally increases statistical power by accounting for some of the variance in the dependent variable and thus increasing the ratio of variance explained by the independent variables, adding a covariate into ANOVA also reduces

4176-430: The mean . For a one sample t-test 16 is to be replaced with 8. Other values provide an appropriate approximation when the desired power or significance level are different. However, a full power analysis should always be performed to confirm and refine this estimate. Statistical power may depend on a number of factors. Some factors may be particular to a specific testing situation, but in normal use, power depends on

4248-500: The minimum sample size required so that one can be reasonably likely to detect an effect of a given size (in other words, producing an acceptable level of power). For example: "How many times do I need to toss a coin to conclude it is rigged by a certain amount?" If resources and thus sample sizes are fixed, power analyses can also be used to calculate the minimum effect size that is likely to be detected. Funding agencies, ethics boards and research review panels frequently request that

4320-459: The null at significance level α = 0.05 {\displaystyle \alpha =0.05\,} , we must find the critical value t α {\displaystyle t_{\alpha }} such that the probability of T n > t α {\displaystyle T_{n}>t_{\alpha }} under the null is equal to α {\displaystyle \alpha } . If n

4392-415: The observation must be at least that unlikely (perhaps by suggesting a sufficiently large estimate of difference) to be considered strong enough evidence against the null. Picking a smaller value to tighten the threshold, so as to reduce the chance of a false positive, would also reduce power, increase the chance of a false negative. Some statistical tests will inherently produce better power , albeit often at

SECTION 60

#1732891485613

4464-550: The population effect size of sufficient accuracy. For example, a careful power analysis can tell you that 55 pairs of normally distributed samples with a correlation of 0.5 will be sufficient to grant 80% power in rejecting a null that the correlation is no more than 0.2 (using a one-sided test, α  = 0.05). But the typical 95% confidence interval with this sample would be around [0.27, 0.67]. An alternative, albeit related analysis would be required if we wish to be able to measure correlation to an accuracy of +/- 0.1, implying

4536-534: The power for detecting an effect of a given size is related to the variance of the covariate. Since different covariates will have different variances, their powers will differ as well. Additional complications arise when we consider these multiple hypotheses together. For example, if we consider a false positive to be making an erroneous null rejection on any one of these hypotheses, our likelihood of this "family-wise error" will be inflated if appropriate measures are not taken. Such measures typically involve applying

4608-525: The power increases with the values of the effect size θ {\displaystyle \theta } and the sample size n , and reduces with increasing variability σ D {\displaystyle \sigma _{D}} . In the trivial case of zero effect size, power is at a minimum ( infimum ) and equal to the significance level of the test α , {\displaystyle \alpha \,,} in this example 0.05. For finite sample sizes and non-zero variability, it

4680-403: The probability of rejecting H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} when H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} is in fact true, then the power of the test is 1 - β where β is the probability of failing to reject H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} when the alternative H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}}

4752-793: The problem according to our test. Let A i {\displaystyle A_{i}} and B i {\displaystyle B_{i}} denote the pre-treatment and post-treatment measures on subject i {\displaystyle i} , respectively. The possible effect of the treatment should be visible in the differences D i = B i − A i , {\displaystyle D_{i}=B_{i}-A_{i},} which are assumed to be independent and identically Normal in distribution, with unknown mean value μ D {\displaystyle \mu _{D}} and variance σ D 2 {\displaystyle \sigma _{D}^{2}} . Here, it

4824-459: The results. The standard linear regression assumptions hold; further we assume that the slope of the covariate is equal across all treatment groups (homogeneity of regression slopes). The regression relationship between the dependent variable and concomitant variables must be linear. The error is a random variable with conditional zero mean and equal variances for different treatment classes and observations. The errors are uncorrelated. That is,

4896-785: The same false positive rates, but different ability to detect true effects. Consideration of their theoretical power proprieties is a key reason for the common use of likelihood ratio tests . Lehr's (rough) rule of thumb says that the sample size n {\displaystyle n} (for each group) for the common case of a two-sided two-sample t-test with power 80% ( β = 0.2 {\displaystyle \beta =0.2} ) and significance level α = 0.05 {\displaystyle \alpha =0.05} should be: n ≈ 16 s 2 d 2 , {\displaystyle n\approx 16{\frac {s^{2}}{d^{2}}},} where s 2 {\displaystyle s^{2}}

4968-511: The same time, if H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}} defines its own probability distribution for t (the difference between the two distributions being a function of the effect size), the power of the test would be the probability, under H 1 {\displaystyle H_{1}} , that the sample t falls into our defined rejection region and causes H 0 {\displaystyle H_{0}} to be correctly rejected. Statistical power

5040-467: The simulated T n {\displaystyle T_{n}} and use that as an estimate of t α {\displaystyle t_{\alpha }} . 4. Now generate a large number of sets of D n {\displaystyle D_{n}} according to the alternative hypothesis, N ( θ , σ D ) {\displaystyle N(\theta ,\sigma _{D})} , and compute

5112-406: The statistical analysis of the collected data to estimate the power will result in uninformative and misleading values. In particular, it has been shown that post-hoc "observed power" is a one-to-one function of the p -value attained. This has been extended to show that all post-hoc power analyses suffer from what is called the "power approach paradox" (PAP), in which a study with a null result

5184-469: Was a significant main effect , it means that there is a significant difference between the levels of one categorical IV, ignoring all other factors. To find exactly which levels are significantly different from one another, one can use the same follow-up tests as for the ANOVA. If there are two or more IVs, there may be a significant interaction , which means that the effect of one IV on the DV changes depending on

#612387